2016
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01779
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mimological Reveries? Disconfirming the Hypothesis of Phono-Emotional Iconicity in Poetry

Abstract: The present study retested previously reported empirical evidence suggesting an iconic relation between sound and emotional meaning in poetry. To this end, we analyzed the frequency of certain phoneme classes in 48 German poems and correlated them with ratings for emotional classification. Our analyses provide evidence for a link between the emotional classification of poems (joyful vs. sad) and the perception of tonal contrast as reflected in the attribution of phenomenological sound qualia (bright vs. dark).… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
0
14
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, another study reported that native speakers rated German, Chinese, Russian, and Ukrainian poems with a relatively high frequency of nasals as sad, and poems with a high frequency of plosives as happy/joyful (Auracher et al, 2010 ). A more recent study (Kraxenberger and Menninghaus, 2016a ), however, could not replicate Auracher et al's ( 2010 ) findings and consequently questions a consistent frequency effect. Thus, it seems that the frequently assumed nexus between sound and emotion perception in poetry (e.g., Valéry, 1958 ; Fónagy, 1961 ; Tsur, 1992 ; Whissell, 2002 , 2011 ) might not consistently be driven by the frequency of occurrence of certain phoneme classes.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…Similarly, another study reported that native speakers rated German, Chinese, Russian, and Ukrainian poems with a relatively high frequency of nasals as sad, and poems with a high frequency of plosives as happy/joyful (Auracher et al, 2010 ). A more recent study (Kraxenberger and Menninghaus, 2016a ), however, could not replicate Auracher et al's ( 2010 ) findings and consequently questions a consistent frequency effect. Thus, it seems that the frequently assumed nexus between sound and emotion perception in poetry (e.g., Valéry, 1958 ; Fónagy, 1961 ; Tsur, 1992 ; Whissell, 2002 , 2011 ) might not consistently be driven by the frequency of occurrence of certain phoneme classes.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…Aryani and colleagues, for example, found that phonetic features that are statistically overrepresented compared to their frequency of occurrence in everyday language can also predict the emotional tone of poems (Aryani, Kraxenberger, Ullrich, Jacobs, & Conrad, 2016; see also Aryani et al, 2013). Furthermore, similar evidence comes from a comparison between results reported by Auracher et al (2010) and Kraxenberger and Menninghaus (2016). Both studies tested the relation between the average occurrence of certain phonemes (plosives and nasals) and the emotional tone of texts.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…font) or phonological form on the perceived emotional content of the poems. Several lines of work indicate that the perceived emotional content of a poem is at least partially explained by the phonological and / or prosodic content of poems [31,[40][41][42][43][44][45].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%