2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.socnet.2014.07.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Methodological considerations in the use of name generators and interpreters

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
40
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 110 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
40
0
Order By: Relevance
“…From the fully restricted sample, the most frequent network size is one acquaintance (34.6 percent). 4 Past research has discussed the vulnerability of name generators to "interviewer effect" where characteristics of interviewers could affect responses (e.g.Mardsen, 2003;Eagle and Proeschold-Bell, 2015). For this survey, in the case of the network size, the intraclass correlation (ICC) was 0.28, which means that 28 percent of the variance in the network size could be attributed to interviewers' characteristics.…”
mentioning
confidence: 84%
“…From the fully restricted sample, the most frequent network size is one acquaintance (34.6 percent). 4 Past research has discussed the vulnerability of name generators to "interviewer effect" where characteristics of interviewers could affect responses (e.g.Mardsen, 2003;Eagle and Proeschold-Bell, 2015). For this survey, in the case of the network size, the intraclass correlation (ICC) was 0.28, which means that 28 percent of the variance in the network size could be attributed to interviewers' characteristics.…”
mentioning
confidence: 84%
“…Recent work has challenged that assumption, showing that many network measures can be well-estimated with incomplete information (Borgatti et al, 2006; Smith and Moody, 2013). This does not mean that measurement concerns can be ignored (for example, see Marsden, 1993; Brewer and Webster, 2000; Marin and Hampton, 2007; Eagle and Proeschold-Bell, 2015), only that missing data itself does not necessarily invalidate a network study. Still, we are only beginning to understand the practical consequences of missing data for network studies (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To be sure, ego-network data collected as a part of a survey are attractive because their results can be generalized to the wider population; however, the network data they collect are seldom, if ever, as robust as whole network data are in tapping into the nuances of network effects. Moreover, as several recent studies have documented (Eagle and Proeschold-Bell, 2015;Fischer, 2009;Paik and Sanchagrin, 2013), ego-network data can be particularly sensitive, and usually in negative ways, to interviewer effects. Of the studies listed in Table 7 that did not use whole network data, Lindsay's (2006Lindsay's ( , 2007Lindsay's ( , 2008 analysis of evangelical elites is something of an exception.…”
Section: Network and Religion: Looking Forwardmentioning
confidence: 99%