2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.02.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mental and physical effort affect vigilance differently

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
57
1
5

Year Published

2006
2006
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
2
57
1
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, researchers have known for some time that mental and physical effort differ in their systemic catecholamine profiles (Fibiger et al, 1984) and in their lasting effects on subsequent task performance (Smit et al, 2005). Critically, at least one study has dissociated regions in humans that process different types of effort costs, with physical effort exertion encoded by motor cortex, and cognitive effort exertion encoded by dorsolateral prefrontal and inferior parietal cortices (Schmidt et al, 2012), targets of future rCET studies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, researchers have known for some time that mental and physical effort differ in their systemic catecholamine profiles (Fibiger et al, 1984) and in their lasting effects on subsequent task performance (Smit et al, 2005). Critically, at least one study has dissociated regions in humans that process different types of effort costs, with physical effort exertion encoded by motor cortex, and cognitive effort exertion encoded by dorsolateral prefrontal and inferior parietal cortices (Schmidt et al, 2012), targets of future rCET studies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More especially, limited cognitive working memory resources in the morning, as a consequence of low alertness, would have been insufficient to enable efficient strategies in the high difficulty/high time pressure conditions, while no effect was expected in the easy and/or low time pressure conditions as in these conditions the task could be solved without requiring specific strategies (germane load virtually inexistent). In other controlled laboratory studies, decreased alertness or arousal has also been shown to be associated with lower working memory performance (Fabbri et al 2007;Smit et al 2005), especially in the higher cognitive load condition (Jackson et al 2014). Further, when the same load factors were explored in air traffic controllers performing a mental arithmetic task, time pressure improved performance in the low difficulty task condition, but only when controllers' alertness was high (Mélan and Cascino 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Several authors stressed that multiple indicators of each cognitive load type would provide a broader evaluation of each load type than single items (Debue and van de Leemput 2014;Leppink et al 2014). In this respect, multidimensional subjective rating scales have been developed, and self-ratings have been performed together with objective ratings, including performance measures, eye-tracking components and differential heart rate (Amadieu et al 2009;Debue and van de Leemput 2014;Galy et al 2012;Leppink et al 2014;Smit et al 2005).…”
Section: Interaction Between Intrinsic and Extraneous Cognitive Load mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This shows that response speed of the other participant is not the source of the effect, but rather the actual degree of exerted effort. When exerting effort yourself, this is associated with increased heart rate, increased blood pressure, decreased heart-rate variability, and a perceived increase in arousal (Howells, Stein, & Russell, 2010;Peters et al, 1998;Smit, Eling, Hopman, & Coenen, 2005). However, to our knowledge, no study so far has examined how participants can perceive (physiologic markers related to) the effort exerted by another person.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%