1999
DOI: 10.1006/jecp.1998.2489
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Memory Strength Affects Reporting of Misinformation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

10
61
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 85 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
10
61
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These results also support those of previous behavioral investigations of the misinformation effect in which stronger memory for the original event, usually manipulated by repeating presentation of the original event, resulted in greater resistance to misinformation (Marche, 1999;Pezdek & Roe, 1995;Sutherland & Hayne, 2001). In addition, behavioral and neuroimaging studies have found that during memory retrieval, true memories tend to be associated with greater ratings and activation of sensory details, as predicted by the reality-monitoring framework (Johnson & Raye, 1981; see also .…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…These results also support those of previous behavioral investigations of the misinformation effect in which stronger memory for the original event, usually manipulated by repeating presentation of the original event, resulted in greater resistance to misinformation (Marche, 1999;Pezdek & Roe, 1995;Sutherland & Hayne, 2001). In addition, behavioral and neuroimaging studies have found that during memory retrieval, true memories tend to be associated with greater ratings and activation of sensory details, as predicted by the reality-monitoring framework (Johnson & Raye, 1981; see also .…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…These results suggest that previous findings that good memory for the details of an event is associated with low levels of suggestibility (e.g. Marche, 1999; a Proportions represent the percentage of studies containing significant correlations in the predicted direction. The percentage of studies containing significant correlations in the nonpredicted direction is presented in parentheses.…”
Section: Conclusion and Discussionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…As discussed above, however, children who were confused about the origin of actions evidenced better learning of a task (Ratner et al, 2000), children with an understanding of mind and good memories of multiple sources committed source errors (Welch-Ross, 1999b), and children who confused memories of multiple incidents rarely reported entirely false information (Powell et al, 1999). In other words, Although exposure to non-target sources (e.g., post-event discussions, interviews) can sometimes improve memories and source monitoring of a target event through reinstatement (Marche, 1999;Principe, Ornstein, Baker-Ward, & Gordon, 2000;Roberts, Lamb, & Sternberg, 1999;Warren & Lane, 1995), some errors in children's testimony are the result of confusing target information and suggested information (e.g., Bruck et al, 2000;Poole & Lindsay, 2001a).…”
Section: Expectations Of Child Witnessesmentioning
confidence: 99%