2007
DOI: 10.1080/17437190802280889
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Measuring defensive responses to threatening messages: a meta-analysis of measures

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
73
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 78 publications
(75 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
1
73
0
Order By: Relevance
“…They tended to consider irrelevant the extreme health and risk images used in some alcohol campaigns. This corresponds with research showing that fear appeals must be carefully constructed, because unless people feel motivated to change their behaviour and possess the skills to do so, shocking images may lead people to deny, rather than tackle, threats (Good & Abraham, 2007;Ruiter, Abraham, & Kok, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 50%
“…They tended to consider irrelevant the extreme health and risk images used in some alcohol campaigns. This corresponds with research showing that fear appeals must be carefully constructed, because unless people feel motivated to change their behaviour and possess the skills to do so, shocking images may lead people to deny, rather than tackle, threats (Good & Abraham, 2007;Ruiter, Abraham, & Kok, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 50%
“…Indeed, considering issues relating to ill health poses a great threat to feelings of adequacy (Cohen & Sherman, 2014;Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, 1997). There is much research evidence suggesting that individuals often process personally relevant health-risk information defensively (Good & Abraham, 2007). For example, when exposed to a personally relevant and threatening health-risk message, individuals have been found to respond by denying personal susceptibility and risk (Brown & Smith, 2007;Stuteville, 1970), becoming more critical of the threatening message (Liberman & Chaiken, 1992), rating the message as less accurate (Croyle, Sun & Louise, 1993) and taking less time to read the message (Brown & Locker, 2009).…”
Section: Application Of Sat To Personally Relevant Health-risk Informmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such defensive responses can potentially limit the efficacy of health promotion campaigns. Indeed, research has frequently reported that individuals who are at greatest risk of engaging in health-detrimental behaviours, i.e., those whom the health promotion campaigns are targeting, are the ones who are most likely to respond defensively to personally relevant health-risk information (Block & Williams, 2006;Good & Abraham, 2007;Keller, 1999;Sherman, Nelson & Steele, 2000;van Riet & Ruiter, 2011).…”
Section: Application Of Sat To Personally Relevant Health-risk Informmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, because people are typically motivated to retain their existing beliefs 81 , and because safety concerns for self and close others are a strong motivational forces, using strategies to defend their beliefs is more likely than abandoning or revising them. For instance, people may intentionally avoid threatening information about climate change 82 or avoid making inferences about its personal relevance 83 . Another strategy that people may use to deal with threatening messages is to question or even reject them 70,81 (i.e., they may adopt sceptical beliefs about climate change; see for example ref.…”
Section: Reacting To Threatsmentioning
confidence: 99%