1979
DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.34.7.583
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mazes, maps, and memory.

Abstract: Psychologists studying cognitive processes in animals are particularly interested in the characteristics of memory, the development of cognitive maps, and the use of foraging strategies by predators searching for prey. Laboratory mazes are well suited to study all three of these issues. This article briefly reviews the history of maze testing, focusing on the question of behavioral stereotypy versus behavioral flexibility, and then summarizes the current thinking about memory, maps, and foraging.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

6
200
0
6

Year Published

1994
1994
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 463 publications
(212 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
6
200
0
6
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, rodents trained to turn in one direction in a maze in which the environmental and intramaze cues were irrelevant were slower to learn the task than those trained in a task in which a particular place was rewarded (Hill & Thune, 1952;Scharlock, 1955;Tolman, Ritchie, & Kalish, 1946). These results have been explained by the disturbing effect of extramaze cues in the response group (Blodgett & McCutchan, 1947) or by assuming that rats use a place strategy during initial learning and only shift to a response strategy with continued training (Hicks, 1964;Means & Douglas, 1970;O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978, 1979; N. S. Sutherland & Mackintosh, 1971). This account could also explain why the ego-allocentric and allocentric groups learned most rapidly in the present study.…”
Section: Use Of Allocentric and Egocentric Strategies As Revealed By mentioning
confidence: 76%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, rodents trained to turn in one direction in a maze in which the environmental and intramaze cues were irrelevant were slower to learn the task than those trained in a task in which a particular place was rewarded (Hill & Thune, 1952;Scharlock, 1955;Tolman, Ritchie, & Kalish, 1946). These results have been explained by the disturbing effect of extramaze cues in the response group (Blodgett & McCutchan, 1947) or by assuming that rats use a place strategy during initial learning and only shift to a response strategy with continued training (Hicks, 1964;Means & Douglas, 1970;O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978, 1979; N. S. Sutherland & Mackintosh, 1971). This account could also explain why the ego-allocentric and allocentric groups learned most rapidly in the present study.…”
Section: Use Of Allocentric and Egocentric Strategies As Revealed By mentioning
confidence: 76%
“…Again, however, the transfer tests rule out this possibility as being necessary to performance (see Olton, 1979), because the fishes went to the goal irrespective of their starting point.…”
Section: Use Of Allocentric and Egocentric Strategies As Revealed By mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If the rat made many errors in the dark, it is suggested that the rat used a place strategy. If, however, the rat made few errors, it is suggested that the rat used a response strategy (see Olton, 1979, for review). Errors were recorded with an infrared video camera and scored exactly the same as the 6 previous days of testing.…”
Section: Testingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although there is much evidence of cognitive abilities in animals (e.g., delayed matching to sample- Roberts & Grant, 1976;concept formation-Wasserman, Kiedinger, & Bhatt, 1988;short-term serial memory-Wright, Santiago, Sands, Kendrick, & Cook, 1985;longterm serial memory-Terrace, 2000; formation of cognitive maps- Olton, 1979;numerical discrimination-Brannon & Terrace, 1998;and timing-Gibbon & Church, 1990), the extent of an animal's expertise at those tasks (if any) has not been investigated. Expertise presupposes cognitive ability, but the converse does not follow.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%