2015
DOI: 10.1111/sode.12164
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Maternal Vocal Interactions with Infants: Reciprocal Visual Influences

Abstract: The present study examined the influence of infant visual cues on maternal vocal and facial expressiveness while speaking or singing and the influence of maternal visual cues on infant attention. Experiment 1 asked whether mothers exhibit more vocal emotion when speaking and singing to infants in or out of view. Adults judged which of each pair of audio excerpts (in view, out of view) sounded more emotional. Face-to-face vocalizations were judged more emotional than vocalizations to infants out of view. Moreov… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
78
1
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 95 publications
(81 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
1
78
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Positive emotional expressiveness, such as happy‐sounding speaking or singing voices (Corbeil et al., ; Kitamura & Burnham, ; Singh, Morgan, & Best, ) or happy‐looking faces (Kim & Johnson, ), seems to underlie the attention‐getting consequences of these stimuli. Indeed, happy‐sounding speech and singing induce comparable attention capture (Corbeil et al., ; Costa‐Giomi & Ilari, ; Trehub, Plantinga, & Russo, ), as do happy‐looking ID and AD faces (Kim & Johnson, ). Attention capture is greater, however, to audiovisual renditions of maternal singing than to maternal speech (Nakata & Trehub, ), perhaps because mothers smile more when they sing than when they speak to infants (Trehub et al., ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Positive emotional expressiveness, such as happy‐sounding speaking or singing voices (Corbeil et al., ; Kitamura & Burnham, ; Singh, Morgan, & Best, ) or happy‐looking faces (Kim & Johnson, ), seems to underlie the attention‐getting consequences of these stimuli. Indeed, happy‐sounding speech and singing induce comparable attention capture (Corbeil et al., ; Costa‐Giomi & Ilari, ; Trehub, Plantinga, & Russo, ), as do happy‐looking ID and AD faces (Kim & Johnson, ). Attention capture is greater, however, to audiovisual renditions of maternal singing than to maternal speech (Nakata & Trehub, ), perhaps because mothers smile more when they sing than when they speak to infants (Trehub et al., ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, happy‐sounding speech and singing induce comparable attention capture (Corbeil et al., ; Costa‐Giomi & Ilari, ; Trehub, Plantinga, & Russo, ), as do happy‐looking ID and AD faces (Kim & Johnson, ). Attention capture is greater, however, to audiovisual renditions of maternal singing than to maternal speech (Nakata & Trehub, ), perhaps because mothers smile more when they sing than when they speak to infants (Trehub et al., ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…23 They promote these goals through animated play, including the singing of play songs. 9,18,24 Such singing is also multimodal, doi: 10.1111/nyas.12622 with mothers smiling as they sing, 25 often with distinctive actions. 24 In other words, singing is almost invariably heard and seen, with touch and movement included on many occasions.…”
Section: Singing To Infants: Cross-cultural Perspectivesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In general, mothers smile more when they sing than when they speak, which may account for infants' greater visual attention to silent videos of infant-directed singing than to comparable videos of speech. 25 Infants also exhibit prolonged attention to audiovisual samples of playful maternal singing relative to playful maternal speech. 29 Acoustic analyses of infant-directed speech reveal more features in common with singing than with conventional or adult-directed speech.…”
Section: Infant-directed Singing: Consequences For Infant Attentionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation