2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.03.026
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Maternal deprivation and the development of stereotypic behaviour

Abstract: Many farm, laboratory, zoo and companion animals experience some form of maternal deprivation. This is typically via separation from their mothers earlier than would happen in free-living populations, in some cases even while young are still dependent upon milk. Maternal deprivation may also occur in a qualitative way, via inadequate maternal care, perhaps caused by inexperience or by restrictive environments that limit maternal behaviours. In this paper we review evidence on the link between early separation … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

8
126
0
6

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 144 publications
(140 citation statements)
references
References 152 publications
8
126
0
6
Order By: Relevance
“…As might be expected from the accounts above, providing enrichments but then removing them often exacerbates or even induces stereotypic behaviour; for instance removing the mother often triggers stereotypic attempts to escape or suckle in young mammals (reviewed [21]), while removing or delaying expected food rewards exacerbates pacing, weaving and sham-chewing in captive pigs and carnivores [16,23]. Less expected is that in some of these cases, the stereotypic behaviours of animals from whom complex natural stimuli are removed become even more prevalent and severe than those of subjects that have lived without enrichment all their lives.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…As might be expected from the accounts above, providing enrichments but then removing them often exacerbates or even induces stereotypic behaviour; for instance removing the mother often triggers stereotypic attempts to escape or suckle in young mammals (reviewed [21]), while removing or delaying expected food rewards exacerbates pacing, weaving and sham-chewing in captive pigs and carnivores [16,23]. Less expected is that in some of these cases, the stereotypic behaviours of animals from whom complex natural stimuli are removed become even more prevalent and severe than those of subjects that have lived without enrichment all their lives.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Scores on one of our measures of perseveration, SA, also predicted the duration of time engaged in SB. These behavioural results thus suggest that, like deprivation-reared primates, and like deer mice developing in standard versus enriched housing conditions (Lewis et al 2006;Latham & Mason 2008), forebrain development is altered in CB animals in a manner that decreases both behavioural flexibility and the ability to inhibit inappropriate behavioural repetitions, with perseveration and SB being two outcomes of such a change. A second factor associated with SB was activity.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…These findings suggest that both age at capture and sex may be important determinants of the behavioural phenotype of WC animals in laboratories, zoos and conservation breeding centres. Furthermore, since all the striped mice, even juvenile-caught, were captured post weaning age (after which time grassland striped mice are solitary; Schradin & Pillay 2005), this suggests that the physical aspects of environmental space and complexity, and/or an extended period of autonomous decision making, may underlie the effects of birth origin and not, for instance, that of the premature loss of parental care (a common confound in other CBeWC comparisons ;Mason 2006;Latham & Mason 2008). Instead of being stereotypic, WC striped mice were extremely inactive within their home cages, spending most of their time in their nestboxes when they otherwise would be expected to be active.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations