1934
DOI: 10.1037/h0074350
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Massed and distributed practice in puzzle solving.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
24
0

Year Published

1956
1956
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Using himself as a subject, Ebbinghaus determined that distributing learning sessions of nonsense syllables with controlled response trials was superior to similar stimuli learned in one session. The superiority of distributing learning and practice over massed learning was replicated by many early researchers (Cook, 1934(Cook, , 1944Jost, 1897;Reed, 1924;Ruch, 1928;Thorndike, 1912).…”
Section: Distributed Practicementioning
confidence: 91%
“…Using himself as a subject, Ebbinghaus determined that distributing learning sessions of nonsense syllables with controlled response trials was superior to similar stimuli learned in one session. The superiority of distributing learning and practice over massed learning was replicated by many early researchers (Cook, 1934(Cook, , 1944Jost, 1897;Reed, 1924;Ruch, 1928;Thorndike, 1912).…”
Section: Distributed Practicementioning
confidence: 91%
“…While the studies reviewed above suggest that it is worthwhile to space the learning of complex skills, this does not necessarily apply to other task domains. Indeed, a number of older studies assessing puzzle learning using spacing intervals of 1 day failed to find an advantage for spaced practice for learning ( Cook, 1934 ; Garrett, 1940 ; Ericksen, 1942 ). For example, Garrett (1940) compared participants who learned a symbol-digit substitution task, a code-learning task or an artificial language task by spaced practice or massed practice.…”
Section: The Spacing Effect In Skill-related Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This confound is problematic because spacing usually boosts subsequent test performance—indeed, this spacing effect is one of the most robust effects in the area of learning (for reviews, see, for example, Cepeda, Pashler, Vul, Wixted, & Rohrer, 2006; Dempster, 1987; Bjork, 1994; Donovan & Radosevich, 1999; Rohrer, 2009; for experimental papers, see, for example, Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, L. E., Bahrick, A. S., & Bahrick, P. E., 1993; Bloom & Shuell, 1981; Cepeda, Vul, Rohrer, Wixted, & Pashler, 2008; Cook, 1934; Kornell & Bjork, 2008; Lee & Genovese, 1988; Mumford et al, 1994; Pyc & Rawson, 2007; Rea & Modigliani, 1985; Rohrer & Taylor, 2006; Seabrook, Brown, & Solity, 2005).…”
Section: A Confoundmentioning
confidence: 99%