2015
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004172
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mapping Shape to Visuomotor Mapping: Learning and Generalisation of Sensorimotor Behaviour Based on Contextual Information

Abstract: Humans can learn and store multiple visuomotor mappings (dual-adaptation) when feedback for each is provided alternately. Moreover, learned context cues associated with each mapping can be used to switch between the stored mappings. However, little is known about the associative learning between cue and required visuomotor mapping, and how learning generalises to novel but similar conditions. To investigate these questions, participants performed a rapid target-pointing task while we manipulated the offset bet… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
30
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
(43 reference statements)
1
30
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For learning associations between colour cues and rotated visuomotor mappings, there is evidence that learning is dependent on whether the participant had explicit knowledge about this relationship (Hegele and Heuer 2010). Also for learning visuomotor shifts, there is evidence that awareness of the cues plays a dominant role for whether or not learning occurs (van Dam and Ernst 2015). The same could be true when there are no associative cues, as in the present study.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 66%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For learning associations between colour cues and rotated visuomotor mappings, there is evidence that learning is dependent on whether the participant had explicit knowledge about this relationship (Hegele and Heuer 2010). Also for learning visuomotor shifts, there is evidence that awareness of the cues plays a dominant role for whether or not learning occurs (van Dam and Ernst 2015). The same could be true when there are no associative cues, as in the present study.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 66%
“…That is, there were no additional associative cues indicating the required mappings in an a priori fashion as was the case in many previous studies (e.g. Kravitz 1972;Kravitz andYaffe 1972, 1974;Martin et al 1996;Seidler et al 2001;Hay and Pick 1966;Pick et al 1969;Donderi et al 1985;van Dam et al 2013;van Dam and Ernst 2015;Welch 1971). Without associative contexts, there is in principle no need for any long-term learning to occur, since the feedback is only provided after the movement is already complete and thus cannot provide a priori knowledge about the currently correct mapping.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In principle, previously learned tool mappings could be identified via a range of cues, all of which were available in our experiments. For example, ‘contextual’ cues, such as the colour/appearance of the tool, could allow switching before movement even begins (Imamizu et al 2007; van Dam and Ernst 2015a). Similarly, static spatial information about hand opening and the tool tips could allow identification.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Stimuli were 5-fold radially symmetric flower-like shapes, as described in van Dam and Ernst, 2015. These were selected on the basis of previous psychophysical testing demonstrating that they are perceptually linear, and the fact that they can be individually generated along axis a single perceptual axis of ‘spikiness’ using the mathematical description provided in the paper.…”
Section: Designmentioning
confidence: 99%