2016
DOI: 10.1002/icd.2014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mapping children's and adolescents' judgment rules for assessing the risk of disease transmission from sick friends

Abstract: Knowing the way children and adolescents assess the risk of disease transmission is important because this kind of knowledge may allow health caregivers to better communicate with them. We had 587 students in Spain and France aged 7–16 judge the risk of disease transmission in 28 scenarios of students visiting a sick friend. The scenarios were composed according to a three within‐subject orthogonal design: type of contact (from no contact to prolonged close contact), type of disease (contagious vs. noncontagio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…By 7 years of age, children use information about the type of disease (contagious vs. not contagious), type of contact (varying levels of physical contact), and frequency of contact to make predictions about the risk of contagion from a sick person (Muñoz Marco et al, 2017).…”
Section: Young Children's Ability To Make Predictions About Novel Illnessesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…By 7 years of age, children use information about the type of disease (contagious vs. not contagious), type of contact (varying levels of physical contact), and frequency of contact to make predictions about the risk of contagion from a sick person (Muñoz Marco et al, 2017).…”
Section: Young Children's Ability To Make Predictions About Novel Illnessesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When provided with competing explanations about why someone got sick, they are more likely to select biological explanations for illness (e.g., explanations that invoke germs or bodily fluids) than explanations that rely on desires or punishment for bad behavior (Hatano & Inagaki, 2013; Kalish, 1997; Siegal, 1988; Slaughter & Lyons, 2003; Springer & Ruckel, 1992). By 7 years of age, children use information about the type of disease (contagious vs. not contagious), type of contact (varying levels of physical contact), and frequency of contact to make predictions about the risk of contagion from a sick person (Muñoz Marco et al, 2017). Perhaps, children can derive a simple strategy from these early conceptual foundations to inform their predictions about illness, which would not require a detailed understanding of germs, invisible particles, or causality.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All five elements had a significant impact on judgments of well-being, and two well-being judgment positions were identified, with each characterized by different cognitive processes (an additive rule in the moderate well-being position and a disjunctive rule in the high well-being position), and with both associated with the participants' age. Muñoz Marco et al (2017) mapped how children and adolescents judged the risk of catching a disease from sick friends. They estimated this risk transmission in scenarios that were constituted from information about type of contact, type of disease, and number of contacts, and they identified six risk judgment positions.…”
Section: An Information Integration Approachmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The information integration approach has also been used to map different judgment and decision positions in sport (e.g., Fruchart & Rulence-Pâques, 2020) and different views of risk in the health domain (e.g., Muñoz Marco et al, 2017). Fruchart and Rulence-Pâques (2020) mapped how adolescents, young adults, and middle-aged adults cognitively combined five elements when estimating the level of well-being in sport.…”
Section: Studiedmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation