1974
DOI: 10.3758/bf03199177
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Magnitude of fear as a function of expected time to all aversive event

Abstract: Three rats were trained to press a lever on a random-interval Lmin schedule of food reinforcement. In successive phases of the experiment, electric shocks were superimposed at I-min fixed intervals, 2-min fixed intervals, or at I-min random intervals. In the fixed-interval conditions, there was a steep gradient of reduction in response rate as the time for the next shock approached; in the random-interval condition, the response rate following a shock was relatively constant. The present method appears to be a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

4
35
0

Year Published

1977
1977
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
4
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Such temporally regulated behavior has also been observed, although with more difficulty, in aversively motivated Pavlovian paradigms such as conditioned barpress suppression (Estes & Skinner, 1941), a widely used procedure in which fear triggered by a CS that was previously paired with a footshock US is assessed by measuring the degree to which the CS disrupts baseline barpressing for food or water according to a variable-interval (VI) schedule of reinforcement (see Hammond & Maser, 1970;Hendry & Van-Toller, 1965;Holmes, Jackson, & Byrum 1971;Labarbera & Church, 1974;Libby & Church, 1975;Rosas & Alonso, 1996, 1997a, 1997bSchachtman, Channell, & Hall 1987;Zielinski, 1966).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such temporally regulated behavior has also been observed, although with more difficulty, in aversively motivated Pavlovian paradigms such as conditioned barpress suppression (Estes & Skinner, 1941), a widely used procedure in which fear triggered by a CS that was previously paired with a footshock US is assessed by measuring the degree to which the CS disrupts baseline barpressing for food or water according to a variable-interval (VI) schedule of reinforcement (see Hammond & Maser, 1970;Hendry & Van-Toller, 1965;Holmes, Jackson, & Byrum 1971;Labarbera & Church, 1974;Libby & Church, 1975;Rosas & Alonso, 1996, 1997a, 1997bSchachtman, Channell, & Hall 1987;Zielinski, 1966).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With short response components, over which a temporal discrimination might better have been established, a time-dependent response suppression effect was found. LaBarbera and Church (1974) found a similar temporal decay in food response rates with shock at regular intervals. The rate of responding soon after shock was higher for ICS but decayed at a similar rate for both food and ICS.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 54%
“…Instead, Group MID and, especially, Group END reached their highest levels of freezing at some later point in the test session (Periods2 and 5, respectively). This patternof resultscan be interpreted as beingdue to temporal conditiorting during the trairting phase(for literature on timingbehavior, see, e.g., Church, 1989;LaBarbera & Church, 1974;Libby& Church, 1975). Notably, in bothGroupsMID and END, the anticipated time of occurrence of shock during the test session (as demonstrated by the enhanced freezing during Periods 2 and 5 for Groups MID and END, respectively) was two 3-min periods ahead of the actual occurrence of the shock during the conditioning session (Periods 4 and 7 for Groups MID and END, respectively).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%