2013
DOI: 10.1242/jeb.076208
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Maggot learning and Synapsin function

Abstract: SummaryDrosophila larvae are focused on feeding and have few neurons. Within these bounds, however, there still are behavioural degrees of freedom. This review is devoted to what these elements of flexibility are, and how they come about. Regarding odour-food associative learning, the emerging working hypothesis is that when a mushroom body neuron is activated as a part of an odour-specific set of mushroom body neurons, and coincidently receives a reinforcement signal carried by aminergic neurons, the AC-cAMP-… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
61
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(62 citation statements)
references
References 117 publications
1
61
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The current working hypothesis for associative olfactory learning in fruit flies and larvae locates the memory trace in the output synapses of the mushroom body Kenyon cells (for review, see Heisenberg 2003;Gerber et al 2009;Diegelmann et al 2013;Schleyer et al 2013). The Kenyon cells receive on the one hand odor information, on the other hand reward and punishment signals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The current working hypothesis for associative olfactory learning in fruit flies and larvae locates the memory trace in the output synapses of the mushroom body Kenyon cells (for review, see Heisenberg 2003;Gerber et al 2009;Diegelmann et al 2013;Schleyer et al 2013). The Kenyon cells receive on the one hand odor information, on the other hand reward and punishment signals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The larva of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster arguably is such a model organism. It is a well-established study case in particular for navigation tasks with respect to olfactory cues (Cobb 1999;Louis et al 2008;Gomez-Marin et al 2011;Lahiri et al 2011;Gershow et al 2012;Louis 2012, 2014;Schulze et al 2015;Wystrach et al 2016) as well as for odor-tastant learning (Scherer et al 2003;Gerber et al 2009;Diegelmann et al 2013;Schleyer et al 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At which stage along the olfactory pathway may such dissociation be found? We first briefly review the architecture of the olfactory pathway (for reviews, see Vosshall, 2000;Gerber and Stocker, 2007;Vosshall and Stocker, 2007;Stocker, 2008;Gerber et al, 2009;Masse et al, 2009;Touhara and Vosshall, 2009;Diegelmann et al, 2013) and then suggest two alternative scenarios for intensity learning.…”
Section: Possible Circuitry Underlying Intensity Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We tackle this issue using odour-sugar associative conditioning in larval Drosophila (Fig.1A) (Scherer et al, 2003;Michels et al, 2005;Neuser et al, 2005;Mishra et al, 2010;Chen et al, 2011;Michels et al, 2011;Saumweber et al, 2011a;Saumweber et al, 2011b) (for reviews, see Gerber and Stocker, 2007;Gerber et al, 2009;Diegelmann et al, 2013). This is a suitable system for such a study due to its simplicity in terms of cell number, its genetic tractability and the robustness of the paradigm.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Learning and memory finetune the way an animal can act in its environment, e.g., in the search for food. Using odor-sugar reward associative learning in larval Drosophila as a study case, we investigate the role of the Synapsin protein in learning and memory (Scherer et al 2003;Neuser et al 2005;Saumweber et al 2011; for reviews, see Gerber et al 2009;Diegelmann et al 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%