1982
DOI: 10.1117/12.932660
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

<title>Spatial Frequency Analysis Of Three-Dimensional Vision</title>

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
11
0

Year Published

1999
1999
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Above a disparity corrugation spatial frequency of 1 c/d, the optimum carrier spatial frequency is approximately 2.6x the corrugation spatial frequency. These results resolve what at first appeared to be a discrepancy between the results of a number of previous studies [2] , [3] , [4] . The consequence of this finding is that in order to validly compare how disparity sensitivity varies as a function of disparity corrugation, spatial frequency measurements should be done under conditions of comparable, and ideally optimal, conditions.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Above a disparity corrugation spatial frequency of 1 c/d, the optimum carrier spatial frequency is approximately 2.6x the corrugation spatial frequency. These results resolve what at first appeared to be a discrepancy between the results of a number of previous studies [2] , [3] , [4] . The consequence of this finding is that in order to validly compare how disparity sensitivity varies as a function of disparity corrugation, spatial frequency measurements should be done under conditions of comparable, and ideally optimal, conditions.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…We constructed a Disparity Sensitivity Function (DSF) from the estimated neural thresholds. To compare the shape of the 'neural DSF' against the DSF measured in a range of psychophysical studies (Figure 5, Panel D: (Tyler, 1973;Schumer and Ganz, 1979;Pulliam, 1982;Rogers and Graham, 1982;Lankheet and Lennie, 1996;Lee and Rogers, 1997;Bradshaw and Rogers, 1999;Hess et al, 1999;Hogervorst et al, 2000;Tyler and Kontsevich, 2001;Bradshaw et al, 2006;Serrano-Pedraza and Read, 2010;Didyk et al, 2011;Kane et al, 2014;Peterzell et al, 2017)), we extracted reported data using WebPlotDigitizer (software freely available at https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer/) and normalized by dividing each threshold against the lowest threshold in each dataset, forcing each DSF to bottom out at 1. Where both upper and lower limits of disparity sensitivity were measured, the upper limit datapoints were excluded.…”
Section: Estimating Neural Thresholdsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consequently we will experience a greater magnitude and range of relative binocular disparities [4]. It has been proposed that the visual system may process disparity at different disparity spatial scales along separate channels [5], analogous to the channels selective for luminance differences at different luminance spatial frequencies [6]. Using a variety of paradigms to investigate both absolute and relative disparity processing, several authors have provided evidence for at least two [7][8][9][10][11] or more [12] disparity spatial channels for disparity processing, which in turn may rely on distinct sets of luminance spatial channels [13][14][15][16].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%