2020
DOI: 10.2147/cia.s241976
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

<p>Auditory Working Memory Explains Variance in Speech Recognition in Older Listeners Under Adverse Listening Conditions</p>

Abstract: Introduction: Older listeners have difficulty understanding speech in unfavorable listening conditions. To compensate for acoustic degradation, cognitive processing skills, such as working memory, need to be engaged. Despite prior findings on the association between working memory and speech recognition in various listening conditions, it is not yet clear whether the modality of stimuli presentation for working memory tasks should be auditory or visual. Given the modality-specific characteristics of working me… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
15
2

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
(75 reference statements)
1
15
2
Order By: Relevance
“…It is commonly quantified with reading span or digit span tasks that require participants to encode, and manipulate, information in memory over short durations (e.g., read a series of sentences aloud and remember the final word of each sentence; report, following a delay, the order of visually or auditorily presented digits). A large body of work has demonstrated that better WM ability predicts better Speech in Noise (SiN) reception performance (Akeroyd, 2008;Bosen & Barry, 2020;FĂŒllgrabe et al, 2015;Gordon-Salant & Cole, 2016;Kim et al, 2020;Pichora-Fuller, Alain, & Schneider, 2017;Rönnberg et al, 2016;Rudner, Rönnberg, & Lunner, 2011;Wong, Roy, & Margulis, 2009). Similar findings were also recently reported by Lad et al, (2020) using a non-speech-based WM task where participants actively adjusted the frequency and amplitude modulation of a pure tone to match a previously presented token.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 60%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It is commonly quantified with reading span or digit span tasks that require participants to encode, and manipulate, information in memory over short durations (e.g., read a series of sentences aloud and remember the final word of each sentence; report, following a delay, the order of visually or auditorily presented digits). A large body of work has demonstrated that better WM ability predicts better Speech in Noise (SiN) reception performance (Akeroyd, 2008;Bosen & Barry, 2020;FĂŒllgrabe et al, 2015;Gordon-Salant & Cole, 2016;Kim et al, 2020;Pichora-Fuller, Alain, & Schneider, 2017;Rönnberg et al, 2016;Rudner, Rönnberg, & Lunner, 2011;Wong, Roy, & Margulis, 2009). Similar findings were also recently reported by Lad et al, (2020) using a non-speech-based WM task where participants actively adjusted the frequency and amplitude modulation of a pure tone to match a previously presented token.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 60%
“…Speech understanding in noisy environments (e.g. following an announcement at a train station, or a friend's voice in the pub) depends not only on hearing acuity but also on a host of cognitive skills including attention, memory and executive function that support listeners' ability to segregate, track and attend to a 'target' signal among interference (de Kerangal, Vickers, & Chait, 2021;Heinrich, Henshaw, & Ferguson, 2015;Holmes, Zeidman, Friston, & Griffiths, 2021;Kim, Choi, Schwalje, Kim, & Lee, 2020;Lad, Holmes, Chu, & Griffiths, 2020; D. R. Moore et al, 2014;Roberts & Allen, 2016). Identifying the cognitive aspects that affect listening outcomes in crowded scenes is a critical prerequisite for interpreting individual variability and understanding the challenges listeners with different cognitive profiles might face during listening.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Attention is essential for encoding precision or gain: the weighting of sensory input by its reliability [ 113 , 114 ]. Verbal auditory working memory—the “phonological loop”—is integral to degraded speech processing [ 115 , 116 , 117 , 118 ], and selective attention importantly interacts with the verbal short term store to sharpen the precision of perceptual priors held in mind over an interval (for example, during articulatory rehearsal on phonological discrimination tasks: [ 119 ]). Listeners with poorer auditory working memory capacity have more difficulty understanding speech-in-noise, even after accounting for age differences and peripheral hearing loss [ 77 , 120 , 121 ].…”
Section: Factors Affecting Processing Of Degraded Speech In the Hementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies using a reading span task (Daneman & Carpenter 1980) have extensively investigated the association between working memory and NR processing [see review, Souza et al (2015)]. Although it is well-established that working memory capacity affects speech-in-noise performance (Akeroyd 2008; Rönnberg et al 2008; Besser et al 2013; Rönnberg et al 2013; Kim et al 2020), the inconsistent interaction between working memory and NR processing was observed in explaining speech intelligibility or memory performance [see review, Souza et al (2015)]. Specifically, the literature found that listeners with larger working memory capacity tend to have better memory performance (Ng et al 2013; Ng et al 2015) but achieve worse speech intelligibility (Neher 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%