2008
DOI: 10.1075/ni.18.1.09tsa
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

<B>"But I first... and then he kept picking"</B><BR> Narrative skill in Mandarin-speaking children with language impairment

Abstract: This study investigates the narrative skill of school-aged children with language impairment in Taiwan. Twelve children, 6 children with language impairment (LI) and 6 children with typical language development (TLD), aged from 8;0 to 9;5 participated in this study. They were asked to tell three personally experienced stories and the longest one was selected and coded along four dimensions, i.e., narrative structure, conjunction, referential strategies, and discourse context. The revision of the Chinese Narrat… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This result is perfectly explicable within the cumulative conceptual complexity approach to connective acquisition, claiming that causal relations are among the most complex coherence relations, which leads to a relatively late acquisition of causal connectives in typical language development (Evers-Vermeul & Sanders, 2009; Spooren & Sanders, 2008). The finding that children with SLI have trouble adequately expressing causal relations by means of i and a is also compatible with the observation that language-impaired individuals avoid using causal connectives and over-rely on less complex (additive and sequential) connectives for maintaining discourse coherence (Befi-Lopes et al, 2008; Gagarina, 2012b; Tsai & Chang, 2008). Thus, semantic analyses of connective use in children’s discourse are not only informative about differences between impaired and unimpaired populations, but also shed light on the quantitative patterns reported in the literature (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 70%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…This result is perfectly explicable within the cumulative conceptual complexity approach to connective acquisition, claiming that causal relations are among the most complex coherence relations, which leads to a relatively late acquisition of causal connectives in typical language development (Evers-Vermeul & Sanders, 2009; Spooren & Sanders, 2008). The finding that children with SLI have trouble adequately expressing causal relations by means of i and a is also compatible with the observation that language-impaired individuals avoid using causal connectives and over-rely on less complex (additive and sequential) connectives for maintaining discourse coherence (Befi-Lopes et al, 2008; Gagarina, 2012b; Tsai & Chang, 2008). Thus, semantic analyses of connective use in children’s discourse are not only informative about differences between impaired and unimpaired populations, but also shed light on the quantitative patterns reported in the literature (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 70%
“…More specifically, studies targeting relational coherence in the narratives of language-impaired individuals have compared overall frequencies of connectives in the speech of children with and without SLI, sometimes distinguishing between additive, temporal and causal connectives. The findings were rather controversial: some studies report no differences between the groups in overall connective frequencies (Baltaxe & D’Angiola, 1992; Liles, 1985a, 1985b), whereas others found under-use of causal and temporal connectives and over-reliance on less complex additives in the narratives produced by children with language impairment (Befi-Lopes et al, 2008; Gagarina, 2012b; Tsai & Chang, 2008). Additionally, several papers report relatively high rates of erroneous connectives by children with SLI (e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Narrative skills are strongly associated with our conceptual and language development (Morrow, 1985; Vygotsky, 1962), connected to later literacy proficiency and academic achievement (Bishop and Edmundson, 1987; Boudreau and Hedberg, 1999; Chang, 2006; Hedberg and Westby, 1993; Westby, 1984; Wetherell et al, 2007) and socialization skills (Wetherell et al, 2007). Children experiencing language-learning difficulties are particularly susceptible to difficulties with the production of coherent and lucid narratives (Botting, 2002; Norbury and Bishop, 2003; Reilly et al, 2004; Tsai and Chang, 2008). They encounter particular difficulties with organizing stories and in using relevant vocabulary and suitable syntactic structures when generating narratives (Boudreau and Hedberg, 1999; Pearce et al, 2003; Reilly et al, 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%