1997
DOI: 10.1006/obhd.1997.2692
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Looking and Weighting in Judgment and Choice

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
51
0

Year Published

1999
1999
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
1
51
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This dissociation suggests that the bias in dwell duration and the dwell frequency bias might index independent aspects of visual decision making. This dissociation provides support for frameworks of decision making which postulate multiple stages, processes, or strategies (e.g., Payne, 1976;Payne, Bettman, Coupey, & Johnson, 1992;Russo & Leclerc, 1994;Senter & Wedell, 1999;Wedell & Senter, 1997;see Ford, Schmitt, Schechtman, Hults, & Doherty, 1989, for a review). Our finding of gaze bias in dwell duration but not in dwell frequency early in the trial might reflect the operation of an early screening stage, where items are quickly encoded and the set of potentially relevant alternatives is reduced.…”
Section: Looking Preferencementioning
confidence: 85%
“…This dissociation suggests that the bias in dwell duration and the dwell frequency bias might index independent aspects of visual decision making. This dissociation provides support for frameworks of decision making which postulate multiple stages, processes, or strategies (e.g., Payne, 1976;Payne, Bettman, Coupey, & Johnson, 1992;Russo & Leclerc, 1994;Senter & Wedell, 1999;Wedell & Senter, 1997;see Ford, Schmitt, Schechtman, Hults, & Doherty, 1989, for a review). Our finding of gaze bias in dwell duration but not in dwell frequency early in the trial might reflect the operation of an early screening stage, where items are quickly encoded and the set of potentially relevant alternatives is reduced.…”
Section: Looking Preferencementioning
confidence: 85%
“…The range-weighting hypothesis, however, would need some modification to explain why phantom decoy effects occur in choice but not judgment. One such modification derives from the well-documented finding that choice is more lexicographic than judgment (Tversky, Sattath, & Slovic, 1988;Wedell & Senter, 1997). Thus, while range extension may result in only a moderate increase in the corresponding weight of that dimension in judgment, it might lead to a much greater increase in weight for choice, as participants tend to weight attributes less evenly in choice.…”
Section: Explanations For the Phantom Decoy Effectmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…In contrast, in multi-alternative decisions which define a larger decision space, deep encoding of the alternatives may not always be possible given the limited information processing capacity of the decision maker. As a result, the decision maker might engage in a 'screening' process where weak alternatives are subject to shallow processing and may be excluded from further processing while promising alternatives are processed to a greater extent (Beach, 1993;Russo & Leclerc, 1994;Senter & Wedell, 1999;Wedell & Senter, 1997). Thus, for given decision task, a manipulation that increases the number of decision alternatives would be expected to increase the degree of selectivity with which decision makers process the decision information.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%