2019
DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.13129
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Long‐term movements and home‐range changes: Rapid territory shifts in meerkats

Abstract: Territoriality and stable home ranges are a common space‐use pattern among animals. These ranges provide its inhabitants with important resources and thus favourable territories are associated with an increased fitness. While the role of territory quality and changes of territory ownership have often been investigated, the changes of territorial boundaries have been less studied. Here, we investigated space‐use changes in a social mammal species, applying a novel analytical approach, calculating long‐term diss… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
19
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
(73 reference statements)
0
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although we did not visit every group every day, we were able to establish whether the group changed sleeping burrow in the evening after an intergroup interaction in 295 cases. Since meerkats typically move 0.5-1 km each day, but have territories of 2-5 km 2 , sleeping burrow changes usually result in the group foraging in a different set of locations [43]. We compared the post-interaction rates of burrow movement with a baseline figure calculated by randomly sampling 10 000 group-day observations across the study period and calculating whether the group moved sleeping burrow on that day.…”
Section: Methods (A) Study Population and Data Collectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although we did not visit every group every day, we were able to establish whether the group changed sleeping burrow in the evening after an intergroup interaction in 295 cases. Since meerkats typically move 0.5-1 km each day, but have territories of 2-5 km 2 , sleeping burrow changes usually result in the group foraging in a different set of locations [43]. We compared the post-interaction rates of burrow movement with a baseline figure calculated by randomly sampling 10 000 group-day observations across the study period and calculating whether the group moved sleeping burrow on that day.…”
Section: Methods (A) Study Population and Data Collectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The dispersed nature of prey is reflected in the groups’ movement dynamics: throughout the day meerkat groups typically move in a relatively slow, continuous fashion while simultaneously foraging. Though individuals forage independently, typically 1–10 m from their nearest neighbors 28 , groups remain highly cohesive while navigating 2–5 km 2 territories 29 . Meerkats have a highly developed vocal repertoire 30 and calls have been shown to play an important role in maintaining cohesion 31 , 32 and in initiating rapid travel when relocating 15 , returning to a sleeping burrow 33 , or as a predator avoidance response 34 .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“… There are a number of animal populations with available long‐term life‐history datasets worldwide. Examples of these are a meerkat ( Suricata suricatta ) population that has been monitored in South Africa since 1993 (Kranstauber et al, 2020; The Kalahari Meerkat Project, 2021), booted eagles ( Hieraaetus pennatus ) in Spain since 1998 (Jiménez‐Franco et al, 2020), great tits ( Parus major ) (The Wytham Tit Project, 2016) and red deers ( Cervus elaphus ) (Isle of Rum red deer Project, 2021) in Great Britain since 1947 and 1972, respectively, chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ) in Tanzania since 1960 (Bray & Gilby, 2020), banded mongooses ( Mungos mungo ) in Uganda since 1995 (Banded Mongoose Research Project, 2016; Vitikainen et al, 2019), and mountain goats and bighorn sheep in Canada since the early 1980s (Festa‐Bianchet et al, 2019). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%