2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.05.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Location representation in enclosed spaces: What types of information afford young children an advantage?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
30
4

Year Published

2010
2010
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
2
30
4
Order By: Relevance
“…This idea builds on findings of early metric coding of distance in infants and toddlers (Newcombe, Huttenlocher, & Learmonth, 1999;Newcombe, Sluzenski, & Huttenlocher, 2005). It is consistent with the finding that scalar magnitudes are easier to use for reorientation than are nonscalar magnitudes (Huttenlocher & Lourenco, 2007;Lourenco et al, 2009;Twyman et al, 2009), and it makes contact with a growing literature on the use of relative magnitude for spatial judgments (Duffy, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 2005;Huttenlocher, Duffy, & Levine, 2002) and in spatial scaling tasks (Huttenlocher, Newcombe, & Vasilyeva, 1999;Huttenlocher, Vasilyeva, Newcombe, & Duffy, 2008). As these articles make clear, the use of relative magnitude changes in many ways in the course of development, partly as a result of experience with navigation, but also as a result of many other factors, including learning to count and measure, and having experiences related to quantity such as sharing food.…”
Section: Future Directionssupporting
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This idea builds on findings of early metric coding of distance in infants and toddlers (Newcombe, Huttenlocher, & Learmonth, 1999;Newcombe, Sluzenski, & Huttenlocher, 2005). It is consistent with the finding that scalar magnitudes are easier to use for reorientation than are nonscalar magnitudes (Huttenlocher & Lourenco, 2007;Lourenco et al, 2009;Twyman et al, 2009), and it makes contact with a growing literature on the use of relative magnitude for spatial judgments (Duffy, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 2005;Huttenlocher, Duffy, & Levine, 2002) and in spatial scaling tasks (Huttenlocher, Newcombe, & Vasilyeva, 1999;Huttenlocher, Vasilyeva, Newcombe, & Duffy, 2008). As these articles make clear, the use of relative magnitude changes in many ways in the course of development, partly as a result of experience with navigation, but also as a result of many other factors, including learning to count and measure, and having experiences related to quantity such as sharing food.…”
Section: Future Directionssupporting
confidence: 89%
“…With scalar cues, search by toddlers of 18-24 months was above chance at both the hiding corner and the geometrically identical opposite corner, although with nonscalar cues, search was random. Lourenco, Addy, and Huttenlocher (2009) provide further support for these findings, and Twyman, Newcombe, and Gould (2009) have shown analogous results for mice. These findings undermine the idea that geometric cues are necessary for success.…”
Section: Critiquesupporting
confidence: 69%
“…Lourenco, Addy, & Huttenlocher (2009), on the other hand, replicated their failure of alternating features using red or blue patterns, rather than solid walls. Because of these conflicting results, children’s ability to use of color cues for reorientation remains unclear.…”
mentioning
confidence: 78%
“…Recent computational models show that image-matching processes can account for the primary findings from behavioral studies of reorientation [27], [28], [29], [30] and neurophysiological studies of oriented navigation [29] in non-human animals. Image-matching theories also can explain several findings from studies of children: When children are disoriented in a square room whose alternating walls differ in brightness [31], [32], they can match the stored image of the goal location in accord with these brightness relations, even though they fail to use such relations in a rectangular room with a single wall of contrasting brightness. In a rectangular room with one wall of a distinctive color or brightness [33], the salience of the discrepancy between visual images of longer and shorter walls may be greater than the discrepancy between images of different colored walls, resulting in behavior primarily in accord with wall length rather than wall color.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%