1998
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1415
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Locating the singular point in first-order optical flow fields.

Abstract: The authors investigated how well participants were able to locate the singular point in an expanding or rotating flow field. The participants' responses gave insight into the magnitude and direction of possible deviations from the veridical point and into the reproducibility of the participants' answers. The stimulus window was circular with a radius of 10°. The performance of the participants improved with increasing expansion or rotation of the stimulus. When either the outer rim or the central part of the … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1999
1999
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While this temporal integration seems long, it is not inconsistent with estimates of the time needed to locate the focus of expansion without pursuit, which ranges from 228 ms to 430 ms (Te Pas et al, 1998; Crowell et al, 1990; Hooge et al, 1999) or even up to 3 seconds (Burr & Santoro, 2001). Similarly, temporal integration in the flash lag effect has also been estimated to range from 100 ms to approximately 500 ms (Roulston et al, 2006; Krekelberg & Lappe, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…While this temporal integration seems long, it is not inconsistent with estimates of the time needed to locate the focus of expansion without pursuit, which ranges from 228 ms to 430 ms (Te Pas et al, 1998; Crowell et al, 1990; Hooge et al, 1999) or even up to 3 seconds (Burr & Santoro, 2001). Similarly, temporal integration in the flash lag effect has also been estimated to range from 100 ms to approximately 500 ms (Roulston et al, 2006; Krekelberg & Lappe, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…The optical #ow "eld is a global visual stimulus consisting of a set of elements that appear to expand from a point in the direction of movement (Gibson, 1950). The optical #ow "eld can provide human navigators with primary cues for judging heading and detecting turning (Warren, Mestre, Blackwell & Morris, 1992;Dyre & Andersen, 1997;te Pas, Kappers & Koenderink, 1998;Warren & Hannon, 1988). This may permit navigation by &&dead reckoning'' where continuous integration of translational and angular components allows for estimation of direction and distance from a point of origin (Schiesser, 1986;Loomis et al, 1993).…”
Section: From Perceptual Cues To Cognitive Representationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In such cases, the subjects' performance critically depends on the exact experimental conditions: 2-D vs. 3-D layout of the scene (van den Berg and Brenner, 1994; Palmisano, 1996; Beusmans, 1998; Grigo and Lappe, 1998; Lappe et al, 1999), stimulus duration (te Pas et al, 1998), number of flow field vectors (Warren et al, 1988) and fixation vs. real or simulated eye movements (Royden et al, 1992). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Psychophysical studies have shown that human observers are indeed able to perceive heading in artificial as well as in pseudo-realistic displays (for review see e.g., Lappe et al, 1999 ; Britten, 2008 ). In such cases, the subjects' performance critically depends on the exact experimental conditions: 2-D vs. 3-D layout of the scene (van den Berg and Brenner, 1994 ; Palmisano, 1996 ; Beusmans, 1998 ; Grigo and Lappe, 1998 ; Lappe et al, 1999 ), stimulus duration (te Pas et al, 1998 ), number of flow field vectors (Warren et al, 1988 ) and fixation vs. real or simulated eye movements (Royden et al, 1992 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%