1983
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1983.40-79
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Local Rates of Responding and Reinforcement During Concurrent Schedules

Abstract: The literature was searched for information about the local rates of responding and reinforcement during concurrent schedules. The local rates of reinforcement obtained from the two components of a concurrent schedule were equal when a long-duration changeover delay was used and when many sessions were conducted, except when the two components provided different simple schedules. The local rates of responding were equal under some conditions, but they differed when one component provided a ratio and the other … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
6
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
1
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…On concurrent schedules, the number of responses for a given alternative equals the local response rate, r, multiplied by the time, T, allocated to that alternative. Thus the VR feedback function is N = r*+ T. (Catania, 1963;Herrnstein, 1961Herrnstein, , 1970; for a review see McSweeney, Melville, Buck, & Whipple, 1983), and thus r1 and r2 are effectively constants. When the schedule values 4 and 42 are held constant, D is the difference of the products of constants and is thus a constant itself.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…On concurrent schedules, the number of responses for a given alternative equals the local response rate, r, multiplied by the time, T, allocated to that alternative. Thus the VR feedback function is N = r*+ T. (Catania, 1963;Herrnstein, 1961Herrnstein, , 1970; for a review see McSweeney, Melville, Buck, & Whipple, 1983), and thus r1 and r2 are effectively constants. When the schedule values 4 and 42 are held constant, D is the difference of the products of constants and is thus a constant itself.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If a subject allocates a total of 20 min to this schedule during which its response rate was 100 per minute it would receive approximately 100* .02.20 or 40 reinforcers. Substituting VR feedback functions for N, and N2 into Equation 2 yields dP/dt = f(D) = f((r * * TI)/T -(r2-02*T2)/T2)-(5) Cancelling time values reveals that D = ri.41 -r2*42-Local rates of responding are relatively independent of preference (Catania, 1963;Herrnstein, 1961Herrnstein, , 1970; for a review see McSweeney, Melville, Buck, & Whipple, 1983), and thus r1 and r2 are effectively constants. When the schedule values 4 and 42 are held constant, D is the difference of the products of constants and is thus a constant itself.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is possible, however, that the basic procedure could have yielded matching if other variables were altered. For example, although no COD was used in the Bradshaw studies, it has been argued that a COD should generally be included in concurrent schedule procedures (Catania, 1966;McSweeney et al, 1983). This experiment investigated the addition of a COD to the procedure employed in Experiment 1.…”
Section: Verbal Reportsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many studies of concurrent schedule performance have employed a changeover delay (COD); this is a time period during which no scheduled reinforcement is delivered for a specified time following a change from responding on one alternative to responding on the other, and it is designed to prevent reinforcement of changeover responses. A number of authors have argued that the inclusion of a COD is an important feature of any effective concurrent schedule procedure (see Catania, 1966;McSweeney, Melville, Buck, & Whipple, 1983). Several reviews of the literature have shown that performance of nonhumans conforms closely to the generalized matching law, with values of a falling between 0.5 and 1.3 in the great majority of cases (Baum, 1979(Baum, , 1983de Villiers, 1977;Horne, 1986;Wearden & Burgess, 1982), though procedural variables have been found to affect sensitivity (Taylor & Davison, 1983;Todorov, Oliveira Castro, Hanna, Bittencourt de Sa, & Barreto, 1983).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Alternatively, it may be that the differences between the measures used for concurrent and multiple schedules are symptomatic of fundamental differences between responding on the two schedules. For example, McSweeney, Melville, Buck, and Whipple (1983) found that the local rates of reinforcement obtained from the components of concurrent schedules are approximately equal for most schedules and that the local rates of responding are equal for many others. If this is so, then Equation 1 does not have an interesting application to the local rates of responding and reinforcement during concurrent schedules.…”
Section: Limitations Of the Present Comparisonsmentioning
confidence: 99%