2019
DOI: 10.1038/s41467-019-10295-w
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Listeners form average-based representations of individual voice identities

Abstract: Models of voice perception propose that identities are encoded relative to an abstracted average or prototype. While there is some evidence for norm-based coding when learning to discriminate different voices, little is known about how the representation of an individual's voice identity is formed through variable exposure to that voice. In two experiments, we show evidence that participants form abstracted representations of individual voice identities based on averages, despite having never been exposed to t… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
20
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
1
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…which may also explain the conservative response biases 2 . Nevertheless, as previously discussed, and despite potential floor effects, SF‐Identifiers still outperformed controls, with proportionally more achieving super‐voice‐recogniser status on this test, suggesting that they may develop stronger internal representations for faces (e.g., Bruce & Young, 1986; Valentine, 1991) and voices (e.g., Kreiman & Sidtis, 2011; Lavan, Knight, & McGettigan, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 59%
“…which may also explain the conservative response biases 2 . Nevertheless, as previously discussed, and despite potential floor effects, SF‐Identifiers still outperformed controls, with proportionally more achieving super‐voice‐recogniser status on this test, suggesting that they may develop stronger internal representations for faces (e.g., Bruce & Young, 1986; Valentine, 1991) and voices (e.g., Kreiman & Sidtis, 2011; Lavan, Knight, & McGettigan, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 59%
“…Pitch is also believed to be used in the service of more complicated auditory tasks, such as voice recognition (Latinus and Belin, 2011; McPherson and McDermott, 2018; Lavan et al, 2019) or the perception of stress in spoken language (Shattuck-Hufnagel and Turk, 1996; Cutler et al, 1997). Moreover, we often must estimate the F0 of a sound amid other sounds that have their own F0s.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Listeners were asked to listen carefully and try to memorize the voice and the name (cf. Lavan, Knight, Hazan & McGettigan, 2019; Lavan, Knight & McGettigan, 2019, for studies using a similar training paradigm). For the valenced training groups, an additional vignette was presented as text alongside the name on the screen during playback (e.g., ‘She helped an elderly man cross the road’ and ‘She is very patient when dealing with other people’s problems’ for positive valence; and ‘She is lying about her age in her dating profile’ and ‘She didn't apologise even though she knew she was in the wrong’ for negative valence).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Strikingly, the effects of within‐person variability on voice identity perception were much reduced when listeners were familiar with the voices: Familiar listeners were able to accurately perceive recordings of the same person as a single identity, despite the substantial within‐person variability. These differences in behaviour between the two groups have been ascribed to listeners having access to stable and robust representations of familiar voices, which enables them to link variable stimuli back to a single identity (e.g., Burton, Kramer, Ritchie & Jenkins, 2016; Lavan, Burton et al ., 2019; Lavan, Knight & McGettigan, 2019, for faces).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%