2017
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/w53gd
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Linking language and events: Spatiotemporal cues drive children's expectations about the meanings of novel transitive verbs

Abstract: How do children map linguistic representations onto the conceptual structures that they encode? In the present studies, we provided 3-4 year old children with minimal-pair scene contrasts in order to determine the effect of particular event properties on novel verblearning. Specifically, we tested whether spatiotemporal cues to causation also inform children's interpretation of transitive verbs either with or without the causal/inchoative alternation (She broke the lamp/the lamp broke). In Experiment 1, we exa… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

2
10
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
2
1

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
2
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The findings also extend the literature regarding the causal form-meaning mapping (e.g., Arunachalam & Waxman, 2010;Kline et al, 2017;Naigles, 1990): Children not only infer a causal meaning from these linguistic cues but also use these cues to modify their initial non-causal event representations to include potential causal links between events.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 76%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…The findings also extend the literature regarding the causal form-meaning mapping (e.g., Arunachalam & Waxman, 2010;Kline et al, 2017;Naigles, 1990): Children not only infer a causal meaning from these linguistic cues but also use these cues to modify their initial non-causal event representations to include potential causal links between events.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 76%
“…Children have been shown to make use of this cue starting from the second year of life (e.g., Arunachalam & Waxman, 2010;Fisher, 2002;Göksun et al, 2008;Kline et al, 2017;Lidz et al, 2003;Naigles, 1990). For example, 2-year-olds choose a scene where a bunny pushes down a duck when they hear a pseudo-verb embedded in a transitive frame ("The bunny is gorping the duck") rather than a scene where they both wave one of their arms parallel to one another (Naigles, 1990).…”
Section: Swiss-german and Turkishmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…If, on the other hand, the causal component is what is crucial, we would predict that toddlers would fail to map transitive verbs heard in dialogues to events in which one event participant performed an action, and another changed state, but there was no evident causal relation between them, for example, no spatial and/or temporal contiguity. Work by Bunger and Lidz (2006) and Kline, Snedeker, and Schulz (2017) indicates that toddlers do expect transitive sentences to describe events with spatiotemporal contiguity between the process and result subcomponents, but in neither study was a non-contiguous event pitted against a truly incompatible event such as the synchronous events used in the current study; in both cases non-contiguous and contiguous (canonically causative) events were pitted against each other at test. Thus, an interesting follow-up of these studies would be to see whether the spatiotemporal contiguity is a critical component of the toddler's representation, or simply a preferred interpretation when available.…”
Section: G Ener Al Discussionmentioning
confidence: 71%