2005
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.2005.23-05
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Linear-Nonlinear-Poisson Models of Primate Choice Dynamics

Abstract: The equilibrium phenomenon of matching behavior traditionally has been studied in stationary environments. Here we attempt to uncover the local mechanism of choice that gives rise to matching by studying behavior in a highly dynamic foraging environment. In our experiments, 2 rhesus monkeys (Macacca mulatta) foraged for juice rewards by making eye movements to one of two colored icons presented on a computer monitor, each rewarded on dynamic variable-interval schedules. Using a generalization of Wiener kernel … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

18
292
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 202 publications
(316 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
(79 reference statements)
18
292
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This bias is evidenced by a winstay/lose-shift tendency both here (Fig. 1C) and in similar behavioral contexts Lee et al, 2004;Corrado et al, 2005;Lau and Glimcher, 2005). This initial bias was observed in neuronal activity at the onset of the warning period (Fig.…”
Section: Sci Activity Represents Evolving Saccade Planssupporting
confidence: 72%
“…This bias is evidenced by a winstay/lose-shift tendency both here (Fig. 1C) and in similar behavioral contexts Lee et al, 2004;Corrado et al, 2005;Lau and Glimcher, 2005). This initial bias was observed in neuronal activity at the onset of the warning period (Fig.…”
Section: Sci Activity Represents Evolving Saccade Planssupporting
confidence: 72%
“…5B) and is independent of the overall rate of rewards delivered to the subject. This reflects the fact that in the experiment the sum of baiting rates was kept constant, resulting in an almost constant rate of rewards delivered to the subject (see also Sugrue et al, 2004;Corrado et al, 2005;Lau and Glimcher, 2008). Adaptation to the overall reward rate can be incorporated in our model by making the parameters that control the product of transition rates, the lateral inhibition, the self-excitation, or the variance of the noise, dynamic variables that depend on the overall rate of rewards.…”
Section: Scope Of the Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a dynamic environment, undermatching becomes even more prominent, because after a change in the reward schedule, it takes a few trials for the choice behavior to be shifted according to the new reward schedule. To illustrate how fast the model is able to shift its choice behavior between blocks of trials, following the same method (with a slight modification) used by Corrado et al (2005), we plot the normalized shift (which is the shift per trial normalized by the programmed reward fraction shift) in choice and reward fractions after each block transition (Fig. 9).…”
Section: Choice Behavior Of the Model In A Dynamic Environmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This model provides a good account of monkeys' behavioral data but leaves open mechanistic questions, such as how integration over the income is done, how the time constant for the integration is determined in the circuit, how local fractional income is calculated (which requires two additional computations, addition and division), and how it can be translated to choice probability. In a revision of this model, Corrado et al (2005) replaced the decision rule according to fractional income, by a softmax function of the difference in local incomes. Our model represents a biophysical instantiation of that scenario, except that it uses return rather than income.…”
Section: Mechanisms Of Matching Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%