2016
DOI: 10.1002/jeab.198
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Limits to preference and the sensitivity of choice to rate and amount of food

Abstract: Studies of choice holding food-amount ratio constant while varying food-rate ratio within sessions showed that local changes in preference depend on relative amount of food. The present study investigated whether sensitivity of choice to food-rate ratio and sensitivity to food-amount ratio are independent of one another when food-rate ratios are varied across sessions and food-amount ratios are varied within sessions. Food deliveries for rats' presses on the left and right levers were scheduled according to th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

3
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
(162 reference statements)
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These components with exclusive food might have been less effective somehow in these rapid-change procedures, because such exclusivity of food produces exclusivity of choice in more prolonged-exposure procedures (e.g., Krägeloh, Davison, & Elliffe, 2005). Such an explanation would fail, however, with the results reported by Aparicio, Baum, Hughes, and Pitts (2016), who found a firm upper limit to choice when relative rate and relative amount of food both favored the same alternative. Choice leveled off at a log peck ratio of about 3.5 (11.3:1), when matching predicted much stronger preferences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…These components with exclusive food might have been less effective somehow in these rapid-change procedures, because such exclusivity of food produces exclusivity of choice in more prolonged-exposure procedures (e.g., Krägeloh, Davison, & Elliffe, 2005). Such an explanation would fail, however, with the results reported by Aparicio, Baum, Hughes, and Pitts (2016), who found a firm upper limit to choice when relative rate and relative amount of food both favored the same alternative. Choice leveled off at a log peck ratio of about 3.5 (11.3:1), when matching predicted much stronger preferences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…These mathematical models have often been analyzed using simple methods, without a focus on statistical rigor. Typically, for example, parameter estimates are found by least-squares optimization (e.g., Aparicio, Baum, Hughes, & Pitts, 2016;Baum & Davison, 2014;Davison & McCarthy, 1988), and sometimes hypotheses are simply tested by observing that parameter estimates are close (or far) from a theoretically expected value (e.g., Baum, 1979Baum, , 1983Mullins, Agunwamba, & Donohoe, 1982).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, numerous studies indicate that response allocation in the initial links by pigeons is exquisitely controlled by the reinforcement parameters arranged in the terminal links. Although concurrent-chains procedures have been used with rodents (e.g., Aparicio et al, 2013Aparicio et al, , 2016Aparicio et al, , 2019Ito & Asaki, 1982;Johnson et al, 2013;Orduña et al, 2013;Yates et al, 2018Yates et al, , 2020, this literature is much less extensive than with pigeons. Furthermore, there is lab lore suggesting that choice in rodents does not come under control of within-session changes in reinforcement parameters as readily as choice does in pigeons (e.g., Kyonka, personal communication; we have not been as successful with rats as we have been with pigeons).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%