1992
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.3.414
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lexical development during middle infancy: A mutually driven infant-caregiver process.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
59
0
2

Year Published

2004
2004
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
5
59
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Parental report was more closely related to concurrent observed vocabulary use than to child talkativeness, suggesting that parental report is indeed a measure of child vocabulary use and not simply a global assessment of the child's verbal production. At the same time, parental report on the CDI Short Form in this sample did not appear to be as highly associated with observed word types or with structured assessments as has been reported for middle-class children (Dunham & Dunham, 1992 ;Corkum & Dunham, 1996;Ring & Fenson, 2000). These results warranted further exploration of factors potentially associated with maternal report.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 41%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Parental report was more closely related to concurrent observed vocabulary use than to child talkativeness, suggesting that parental report is indeed a measure of child vocabulary use and not simply a global assessment of the child's verbal production. At the same time, parental report on the CDI Short Form in this sample did not appear to be as highly associated with observed word types or with structured assessments as has been reported for middle-class children (Dunham & Dunham, 1992 ;Corkum & Dunham, 1996;Ring & Fenson, 2000). These results warranted further exploration of factors potentially associated with maternal report.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 41%
“…Early work on the original long-form CDI demonstrated moderate to strong associations between middle-class parents' report of two-year-old children's vocabulary production and concurrent measures of children's spontaneous vocabulary use (Fenson et al, 1994). Corkum & Dunham (1996) report moderate associations between maternal report and lexical word types in spontaneous speech of children aged 1;6 from middleclass families ; similarly, Dunham & Dunham (1992), studying a sample of middle-to upper-middle-class families, found moderately strong associations (r=0 . 71-0 .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Researchers have primarily focused on understanding how joint attention facilitates language development (Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998;Dunham & Dunham, 1992;Dunham, Dunham, & Curwin, 1993;Morales, et al, 2000a;Tomasello & Farrar, 1986), and it is easy to conceptualize how it might do so. Baldwin (1995), for example, demonstrated that when 18-month-old children and an adult experimenter were looking at two different novel objects, and the adult applied a novel word to her own object, children would learn the novel label for the experimenter's object rather than identifying the label with their own object.…”
Section: Nih-pa Author Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Joint attention has been divided into two basic categories: 1) responding to joint attention (RJA) occurs when one follows the direction of eye gaze, head turn, or pointing gesture of a social partner (Mundy, Hogan, & Doehring, 1996), whereas 2) initiating joint attention (IJA) occurs when one points or looks at an interesting object or event while alternating gaze between the object and a social partner (Mundy et al, 1996). Joint attention is most often operationally defined either by the amount of time spent in mutual object engagement in naturalistic settings or by experimentally induced behaviors (i.e., eye gaze following or pointing in response to experimenter prompts).Researchers have primarily focused on understanding how joint attention facilitates language development (Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998;Dunham & Dunham, 1992;Dunham, Dunham, & Curwin, 1993;Morales, et al, 2000a;Tomasello & Farrar, 1986), and it is easy to conceptualize how it might do so. Baldwin (1995), for example, demonstrated that when 18-month-old children and an adult experimenter were looking at two different novel objects, and the adult applied a novel word to her own object, children would learn the novel label for the experimenter's object rather than identifying the label with their own object.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At a more proximal level, specific variations in maternal verbal input and sensitivity or responsivity to children during mother-child interaction have been linked with the rate of children's language development, in both typically developing and at-risk groups (Dale, Greenberg, & Crnic, 1987;Dunham & Dunham, 1992). For instance, in multivariate longitudinal studies of children's vocabulary development, Bornstein and colleagues (Bornstein, 1998) showed that mothers' spontaneous expressive vocabulary to their infants uniquely predicted infants' comprehension at 20 months.…”
Section: Individual Differences and The Effects Of Biological And Socmentioning
confidence: 99%