2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lexical and post-lexical phonological representations in spoken production

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
156
2
4

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 165 publications
(172 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
10
156
2
4
Order By: Relevance
“…they only produced single phoneme errors in all production tasks. This pattern of errors had previously been attributed to underlying "postlexical" impairment (Kohn & Smith 1994;Goldrick & Rapp, 2007). Although the exact definition of "post-lexical" impairment varies between authors, it globally refers to patterns in which the lexeme has been correctly retrieved, but further application of phonological rules and phonetic specification fails, hence resulting in phonological-phonetic impairments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…they only produced single phoneme errors in all production tasks. This pattern of errors had previously been attributed to underlying "postlexical" impairment (Kohn & Smith 1994;Goldrick & Rapp, 2007). Although the exact definition of "post-lexical" impairment varies between authors, it globally refers to patterns in which the lexeme has been correctly retrieved, but further application of phonological rules and phonetic specification fails, hence resulting in phonological-phonetic impairments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…6 The level of representation responsible for frequency effects in speech production remains controversial, however. Most models of phonological planning distinguish at least two stages, the retrieval of a general lemma corresponding with general lexical information and syntactic information associated with words, and a second stage of phonological retrieval and planning (Dell & O'Seaghdha, 1992;Goldrick & Rapp, 2007;Levelt, 1992). Some models locate the role of frequency at the point of phonological retrieval; that is, more frequent phonological word forms are retrieved faster (Levelt et al, 1999).…”
Section: Measures Of Word Probabilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are many measures of prosodic boundary strength, and for simplicity we will focus in this paper on the presence and duration of pauses (Goldman-Eisler, 1972;Grosjean & Collins, 1979;Kendall, 2013;Krivokapić, 2007;Price et al, 1991). In sociolinguistic literature on CSD, a following pause is treated as an environment on par with that of a following consonant or a following vowel.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of studies have documented individuals that violate this prediction (Galluzzi, Bureca, Guariglia, & Romani, 2015;Goldrick & Rapp, 2007;Romani & Galluzzi, 2005;Romani, Galluzzi, Bureca, & Olson, 2011;Romani, Olson, Semenza, & Granà, 2002). Furthermore, the errors of individuals exhibiting this pattern are strongly influenced by the acoustic/articulatory complexity of phonological structures (e.g., exhibiting errors on less-frequent sequences of consonants), but relatively uninfluenced by lexical properties (e.g., word frequency).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%