1999
DOI: 10.3758/bf03198543
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Levels-of-processing effects in subject-performed tasks

Abstract: In memory for subject-performed tasks (SPTs), subjects encode a list of simple action phrases (e.g., thumb through a book, knock at the door) by performing these actions during learning. In three experiments, we investigated the size of the levels-of-processing effects in SPTs as compared with those in standard verbal learning tasks (VTs). Subjects under SPT and VTconditions learned lists of action phrases in a surface or a conceptual orienting task. Under both encoding conditions, the subjects recalled fewer … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

4
43
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
4
43
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast, Engelkamp and Zimmer (1994) focused on the motoric nature of SPTs and assumed that more item-specific information should be provided by SPTs than by VTs, but that the degree of categorical relational information should not differ between SPTs and VTs. The key argument for the claim of better itemspecific information in SPTs was that enactment forces subjects to focus more on the individual items than does verbal learning (see, e.g., Engelkamp, 1995;Zimmer & Engelkamp, 1999) and that encoding of action-specific features enhances the representation of each item (Zimmer, 2001). Categorical relational information should not differ between SPTs and VTs because it is based on preexperimental conceptual knowledge and is encoded by the process of spreading activation among the categorically related representations.…”
Section: Categorical Relational Information In Memory For Action Eventsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, Engelkamp and Zimmer (1994) focused on the motoric nature of SPTs and assumed that more item-specific information should be provided by SPTs than by VTs, but that the degree of categorical relational information should not differ between SPTs and VTs. The key argument for the claim of better itemspecific information in SPTs was that enactment forces subjects to focus more on the individual items than does verbal learning (see, e.g., Engelkamp, 1995;Zimmer & Engelkamp, 1999) and that encoding of action-specific features enhances the representation of each item (Zimmer, 2001). Categorical relational information should not differ between SPTs and VTs because it is based on preexperimental conceptual knowledge and is encoded by the process of spreading activation among the categorically related representations.…”
Section: Categorical Relational Information In Memory For Action Eventsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Zimmer and Engelkamp (1999) found that a conceptual orienting task enhanced memory for nonperformed action phrases more than for performed actions. Actions thus seem to be remembered without the use of intentional encoding strategies.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When a semantic orienting task is assigned, fore-knowledge of the upcoming memory test is irrelevant (Craik, 1977;Jenkins, 1969, 1973). Similarly, instructional manipulations about whether or not memory will tested do not influence recall after enactment (Watanabe, 2003;Zimmer and Engelkamp, 1999). However, the large majority of enactment studies have used intentional instructions, as we do here.…”
mentioning
confidence: 66%
“…A small number of published studies have included some encoding manipulation other than enactment versus intentional-instruction alone, but these have been designed to assess whether the benefit of the other manipulation is additive with the benefit of enactment (Cohen, 1981;Nilsson and Craik, 1990). Zimmer and Engelkamp (1999) asked participants to judge whether a letter triplet occurred in an action phrase (nonconceptual task), or judge whether the described location was a good one for the action (conceptual task, e.g., "apply the postage stamp in the post office" or "…in the pub"). In both cases, the action phrases were performed after the judgment.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%