1981
DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.73.2.212
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Level of analysis in the perception of ongoing instruction: An exploratory study.

Abstract: The level of perceptual analysis at which learners operate during ongoing instruction is suggested to he a variahle mediating the effects of instructional variahles on learner outcomes. Several instructional variables were manipulated to determine whether they influence the level of perceptual analysis. Unitization instructions, lesson difficulty, teacher effectiveness, and teacher order were found to significantly influence perceptual analysis. The relationships of perceptual analysis to concept learning and … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
6
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2010
2010

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
2
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Koopman & Newtson 1981). Further evidence for an effect of mental segmentation differences on learning is provided by studies about design measures that likely disturbed participants' perception of the structure underlying the information shown (Kurby & Zacks 2008;Zacks et al 2007).…”
Section: Enhancing Perception Of the Underlying Structurementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Koopman & Newtson 1981). Further evidence for an effect of mental segmentation differences on learning is provided by studies about design measures that likely disturbed participants' perception of the structure underlying the information shown (Kurby & Zacks 2008;Zacks et al 2007).…”
Section: Enhancing Perception Of the Underlying Structurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Koopman & Newtson 1981). Differences in segmentation rates have been found between participants who were asked to segment videos about several human activities (e.g., a couple playing a party game, a person doing dishes, assembling a saxophone, and fertilizing house plants); that is, there were differences in the size of the substeps or subevents in which participants divided the videos (Hanson & Hirst 1989;Zacks et al, 2006;Zacks et al, 2001).…”
Section: Enhancing Perception Of the Underlying Structurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Others have extended the procedure to more complex behaviors of individuals (e.g., Buck, Baron, Goodman, & Shapiro, 1980;Shapiro, 1982;Zuckerman, Driver, & Guadagno, 11985;Zuckerman, Kernis, Driver, & Koestner, 1984) and to dyadic coaction in assembling tinker toys (Jensen & Schroder, 1982), but in each case, only the number of segments into which perceivers parsed the interaction was considered. In research with interaction specimens, only one interactant's behavior was clearly visible and barsed (Koopman & Newtson, 1981;Russell, 1979;Russell, Gowaty, Harland, & Martin, 1979;Strenta & Kleck, 1984), or the action unit boundaries were lqcated by the investigators themselves (Cronshaw & Lord, 1987). Again, only the number of observer marks was considered.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A more interesting explanation involves the nature of the segmenting task. Koopman and Newtson (1981) instructed subjects to use either a global, natural, or fine-grained level of analysis and found superior recall for the fine-grained group. Cohen and Ebbesen (1979) instructed observers to either look for task information or impression information and found superior task content recall in the task condition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Newtson and his colleagues (e.g., Koopman & Newtson, 1981;Newtson, 1976Newtson, , 1980Newtson & Engquist, 1976) have argued that observers can control the amount of information gleaned from any behaviour sequence by altering their level of analysis. When behaviour is relatively slow changing or relatively predictable, a global level of analysis is appropriate.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%