The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
1979
DOI: 10.1016/s0022-5371(79)90136-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Letter and order information in lexical access

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

21
139
2
4

Year Published

1986
1986
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 158 publications
(166 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
21
139
2
4
Order By: Relevance
“…This replicates the classic transposed-letter effect in hearing readers in a lexical decision task, where transposed-letter nonwords have higher latency response and error rate than replaced-letter nonwords (Chambers, 1979;O'Connor & Forster, 1981), and extends the findings to deaf skilled readers. Previous studies have suggested that the transposed-letter effect reflects orthographic processing (Perea & Carreiras, 2006), and the current results therefore demonstrate that deaf skilled readers are as sensitive as hearing controls to orthographic processes.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…This replicates the classic transposed-letter effect in hearing readers in a lexical decision task, where transposed-letter nonwords have higher latency response and error rate than replaced-letter nonwords (Chambers, 1979;O'Connor & Forster, 1981), and extends the findings to deaf skilled readers. Previous studies have suggested that the transposed-letter effect reflects orthographic processing (Perea & Carreiras, 2006), and the current results therefore demonstrate that deaf skilled readers are as sensitive as hearing controls to orthographic processes.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…This effect has been observed both in adults (e.g., Andrews, 1996;Acha & Perea, 2008b, 2010Bruner & O'Dowd, 1958;Chambers, 1979;Christianson, Johnson, & Rayner, 2005;Forster, Davis, Schoknecht, & Carter, 1987;García-Orza, Perea, & Muñoz, 2010;Holmes & Ng, 1993;Johnson & Dunne, 2012;Kinoshita & Norris, 2009;Lupker, Perea, & Davis, 2008;O'Connor & Forster, 1981;Perea & Acha, 2009b;Perea & Carreiras, 2006a, 2006b, 2006cPerea & Lupker, 2003a, 2003bPerea, Mallouh, & Carreiras, 2010;Perea, Duñabeitia, & Carreiras, 2008;Perea & Pérez, 2009;Perea, Winskel, & Ratitamkul, 2012;Schoonbaert & Grainger, 2004;Taft & Van Graan, 1998;Velan & Frost, 2011) and in children (Acha & Perea, 2008a;Castles, Davis, Cavalot, & Forster, 2007;Kohnen & Castles, 2013;Lété & Fayol, 2013;Paterson, Read, McGowan, & Jordan, 2015;Perea & Estévez, 2008;TiffinRichards & Schroeder, 2015). Transposed letter effects have also been reported for silent sentence reading (see Acha & Perea, 2008b;Blythe, Johnson, Liversedge, & Rayner, 2014;Johnso...…”
Section: The Transposed Letter Effectmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…This type of similarity relationship, in which two letter strings differ with respect to a single pair of adjacent letters, is known as transposed-letter similarity. Empirical work has shown that transposed-letter similarity affects performance in a variety of reading tasks, including lexical decision, naming, and semantic categorization (e.g., Andrews, 1996;Chambers, 1979;Perea & Carreiras, in press;Perea & Lupker, 2003, 2004Taft & van Graan, 1998). Selecting the TL field causes the program to check whether the input string is a member of a transposed-letter pair-that is, whether a word can be formed by transposing an adjacent pair of letters in the input string.…”
Section: Phonological Statisticsmentioning
confidence: 99%