1976
DOI: 10.1016/s0010-9452(76)80008-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Left-Handedness and Cognitive Deficit

Abstract: Intellectual and performance measures were taken on 7688 school children tested on three behavioral measures of handedness and one measure of eyedness. Test results were compared against all combinations of handedness and eyedness and against a measure of socio-economic level. No relationships of any kind were found. Comparisons of the present results are made against 33 studies concerned with possible deficits associated with left-handedness. The results of the present study combined with a review of the majo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
66
1
2

Year Published

1990
1990
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 230 publications
(75 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
6
66
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Cultural transmission provides a relatively quick means for adapting to a wide range of ecologies and environmental changes. An explanation based upon wholly cultural processes is also compatible with evidence documenting relatively rapid changes in cultural values (i.e., changes observed over the course of a generation or two) associated with immigrant populations who migrate from ecologies of high disease threat to ecologies with substantially lower disease threat (e.g., Hardyck, Petrinovich, & Goldman, 1976). Humans are also equipped with cognitive tools and biases which are compatible with a cultural transmission explanation, such as perceptual hypervigilance to cues connoting the presence of threat, selective communication about threat, and social learning of avoidant responses to threat (Cook & Mineka, 1990;Öhman, Flykt, & Esteves 2001;Schaller & Conway, 1999).…”
Section: Cultural Transmission Processessupporting
confidence: 69%
“…Cultural transmission provides a relatively quick means for adapting to a wide range of ecologies and environmental changes. An explanation based upon wholly cultural processes is also compatible with evidence documenting relatively rapid changes in cultural values (i.e., changes observed over the course of a generation or two) associated with immigrant populations who migrate from ecologies of high disease threat to ecologies with substantially lower disease threat (e.g., Hardyck, Petrinovich, & Goldman, 1976). Humans are also equipped with cognitive tools and biases which are compatible with a cultural transmission explanation, such as perceptual hypervigilance to cues connoting the presence of threat, selective communication about threat, and social learning of avoidant responses to threat (Cook & Mineka, 1990;Öhman, Flykt, & Esteves 2001;Schaller & Conway, 1999).…”
Section: Cultural Transmission Processessupporting
confidence: 69%
“…Left-handers are found at lower frequencies in societies that associate it with clumsiness, evil, dirtiness or mental illness, such as some middle and far eastern countries (Harris 1980;Corballis 1991). Studies of school children in China and Taiwan report only 3.5 and 0.7 per cent used their left hand for writing, compared with a 6.5 per cent estimate for Oriental school children living in the USA (Hardyck et al 1976;Teng et al 1976;Hung et al 1985).…”
Section: Case Studies Of Gene-culture Coevolutionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Firstly, there are numerous findings that left-handedness is associated with various undesirable outcomes such as low cognitive ability (e.g. Hardyck, Petrinovich, & Goldman 1976, McManus & MascieTaylor 1983 as well as a number of unusual and sometimes pathological conditions. For example a higher incidence of left handedness is found amongst groups with a history of alcoholism, autism, criminality, depression, homosexuality, immune diseases, psychosis and schizophrenia to mention but a few 6 .…”
Section: Lateralitymentioning
confidence: 99%