2001
DOI: 10.1080/02687040143000050
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning of subordinate category names by aphasic subjects: A comparison of deep and surface-level training methods

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The mapping that needs to be primed in word retrieval disorders is the output connection between semantics and the lexicon (and from there to phonological representations). Treatments that train semantic features of words are presumably stimulating this pathway, as are self-generated semantic cues, and these approaches have met with some success (Boyle & Coelho, 1995;Marshall, Freed, & Karow, 2001). Massed priming of the output semantic-lexicalphonological pathway could be accomplished with a task that promotes naming of the target rather than just repetition.…”
Section: Generalisationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The mapping that needs to be primed in word retrieval disorders is the output connection between semantics and the lexicon (and from there to phonological representations). Treatments that train semantic features of words are presumably stimulating this pathway, as are self-generated semantic cues, and these approaches have met with some success (Boyle & Coelho, 1995;Marshall, Freed, & Karow, 2001). Massed priming of the output semantic-lexicalphonological pathway could be accomplished with a task that promotes naming of the target rather than just repetition.…”
Section: Generalisationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Emphasis has been on establishing the most beneficial therapy technique by looking at one particular therapy with one individual (in a single-case study) and measuring improvement in performance (Francis et al, 2002) or by comparing two different types of therapy in one or more individuals. These include, for example, comparisons of: semantic versus phonological cueing (Chin Li & Williams, 1989;Wambaugh, 2003;Wambaugh, Linebaugh, Doyle, & Martinez, 2001); personalised versus phonological cueing (Marshall, Freed, & Karow, 2001); constraint-induced aphasia therapy versus conventional aphasia therapy (Pulvermuller et al, 2001); decreasing versus increasing cues (Abel, Schultz, Radermacher, Willmes, & Huber, 2005;Conroy, Sage, & Lambon Ralph, 2009); errorless versus errorful therapy (Fillingham, Hodgson, Sage, & Lambon Ralph, 2003;Fillingham, Sage, & Lambon Ralph, 2006).…”
Section: Number Of Words In Anomia Therapy 1065mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is probably because repetition can be accomplished without ever stimulating the top-down connection between semantics and the lexicon. Other tasks that strengthen that activation, such as semantic feature training 21,22 or direct priming of the semantic-lexical connection, should meet with more success.…”
Section: Low Familiarity With Sound or Word Sequencesmentioning
confidence: 99%