2015
DOI: 10.1121/1.4930566
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lateralization of noise bursts in interaurally correlated or uncorrelated background noise using interaural level differences

Abstract: The interaural level difference (ILD) of a lateralized target source may be effectively reduced when the target is presented together with background noise containing zero ILD. It is not certain whether listeners perceive a position congruent with the reduced ILD or the actual target ILD in a lateralization task. Two sets of behavioral experiments revealed that many listeners perceived a position at or even larger than that corresponding to the presented target ILD when a temporal onset/offset asynchrony betwe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
1
1

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Nevertheless, we found that the perceived location of targets was pushed away from the location of the background noise. Although previous studies have observed ‘pulling’ effects between sounds that are grouped together (Lee et al, 2009), our target sounds were clearly distinct from the background noise, which tends to produce ‘pushing’ effects (Suzuki et al, 1993; Canevet and Meunier, 1996; Getzmann, 2002; Best et al, 2005; Reed and van de Par, 2015). We also found greater pushing effects for narrowband targets, which indicates that the pushing effect is a feature of binaural spatial processing.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Nevertheless, we found that the perceived location of targets was pushed away from the location of the background noise. Although previous studies have observed ‘pulling’ effects between sounds that are grouped together (Lee et al, 2009), our target sounds were clearly distinct from the background noise, which tends to produce ‘pushing’ effects (Suzuki et al, 1993; Canevet and Meunier, 1996; Getzmann, 2002; Best et al, 2005; Reed and van de Par, 2015). We also found greater pushing effects for narrowband targets, which indicates that the pushing effect is a feature of binaural spatial processing.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…However, sound localization can be affected even if targets are clearly audible and distinguishable from concurrent sounds. In such circumstances, the perceived location of a target sound is typically pushed away from the location of a concurrent sound (Suzuki et al, 1993; Canevet and Meunier, 1996; Getzmann, 2002; Best et al, 2005; Lee et al, 2009; Reed and van de Par, 2015). Previous attempts to explain this have relied upon a neural map of space that contains an array of neurons (or ‘spatial channels’) tuned to different spatial locations (Suzuki et al, 1993; Best et al, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although a number of ILD models have been proposed (Breebaart et al, 2001;Brown, 2012;Brown and Tollin, 2016;Bures and Marsalek, 2013;Hartmann and Constan, 2002;Lindemann, 1986;Reed and van de Par, 2015;Stern et al, 1988;Takanen et al, 2014), these models mostly did not address the impact of temporal signal properties on ILD perception. The approaches by Brown (2012) and Brown and Tollin (2016) appear particularly interesting with respect to this because they used a physiologically plausible frontend model (Zilany et al, 2014), which has been shown to account for temporal aspects in monaural auditory processing up to the AN.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%