1999
DOI: 10.1016/s0167-8760(99)00085-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Laterality, word valence, and visual attention: a comparison of depressed and non-depressed individuals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

3
11
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
3
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Across multiple early-and late-dependent measures, readers were able to process positive emotion words with greater ease than neutral words. These findings are consistent with previous research that shows that emotional words impact response times in cognitive tasks and are processed more quickly than neutral words (Kakolewski et al, 1999;Kanske & Kotz, 2007;Kousta et al, 2009;Kuchinke et al, 2007;Schacht & Sommer, 2009). Specifically, the current study supported the motivated attention and affective states (Lang et al, 1990(Lang et al, , 1997 proposed by Kousta et al (2009).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Across multiple early-and late-dependent measures, readers were able to process positive emotion words with greater ease than neutral words. These findings are consistent with previous research that shows that emotional words impact response times in cognitive tasks and are processed more quickly than neutral words (Kakolewski et al, 1999;Kanske & Kotz, 2007;Kousta et al, 2009;Kuchinke et al, 2007;Schacht & Sommer, 2009). Specifically, the current study supported the motivated attention and affective states (Lang et al, 1990(Lang et al, , 1997 proposed by Kousta et al (2009).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
“…Several studies have reported significant levels of interference where reactions have increased due to the target being an emotion word. When considering the reading and processing of isolated word stimuli, the findings from several paradigms suggest a processing advantage for positive words (i.e., those with high arousal and high valence) over neutral words (Kakolewski, Crowson Jr., Sewell, & Cromwell, 1999;Kanske & Kotz, 2007;Kuchinke, Võ, Hofmann, & Jacobs, 2007;Schacht & Sommer, 2009;Scott, O'Donnell, Leuthold, & Sereno, 2009), as well as for negative words (i.e., those with high arousal and low valence) over neutral words (Kanske & Kotz, 2007;Kousta, Vinson, & Vigliocco, 2009;Nakic, Smith, Busis, Vythilingam, & Blair, 2006;Schacht & Sommer, 2009;Tabert et al, 2001;Windmann, Daum, & Güntürkün, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This may account for the mixed results on affective word processing. In the emotion word literature, some studies demonstrate a processing advantage for positive over neutral words (e.g., [39][42]), some show an advantage for negative over neutral words (e.g., [17], [41], [43]), and others observe an advantage for positive over negative words (e.g., [44][46]). Such variability could potentially be due to differences in the ratio of intrinsically and extrinsically emotional words presented.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A structural MPRAGE scan was obtained at each session. Six functional scans were acquired during three different affective tasks that consisted of implicit processing of emotional cues (faces) (43), processing of emotion cues (words) in the attentional background (44)(45)(46), and processing of emotional cues (pictures) in the attentional foreground (47) (two scans each: 99-106 images per scan, 620 images per week). The duration of each functional imaging session was 31 minutes, 24 seconds.…”
Section: Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmiamentioning
confidence: 99%