2001
DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/070)
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Language Development and Symbolic Play in Children With and Without Familial Risk for Dyslexia

Abstract: The purposes of this study were to investigate (a) whether children in families with a positive history of dyslexia were more likely to show delays in language development than children without family risk and (b) whether a delayed onset of expressive language (late talking) predicted later language development. We analyzed the language development of 200 children longitudinally at 14, 24, 30, and 42 months and assessed their symbolic play at 14 months. Half of the children (N = 106) were from families with a … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

13
83
1
5

Year Published

2004
2004
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 105 publications
(102 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
(52 reference statements)
13
83
1
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Late talkers are a group of children who learn to form sentences later and have smaller vocabularies than their more typical peers who start putting words together before turning two (Preston et al, 2010). This early language delay has been connected with a risk of later difficulties in language learning such as dyslexia (Lyytinen et al, 2001;Lyytinen, 2015;Lyytinen et al, 2005). A very recent finding (Lyytinen, 2015) confirmed earlier observation (Lyytinen et al, 2005) reveals that if such an early language delay comprises receptive language, i.e.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 75%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Late talkers are a group of children who learn to form sentences later and have smaller vocabularies than their more typical peers who start putting words together before turning two (Preston et al, 2010). This early language delay has been connected with a risk of later difficulties in language learning such as dyslexia (Lyytinen et al, 2001;Lyytinen, 2015;Lyytinen et al, 2005). A very recent finding (Lyytinen, 2015) confirmed earlier observation (Lyytinen et al, 2005) reveals that if such an early language delay comprises receptive language, i.e.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 75%
“…Longitudinal study of dyslexia in (Lyytinen et al, 2001) recognized that late talkers with a familial risk for dyslexia are more likely to have such problems than typical children with the same familial risk.…”
Section: Late Talkersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Converging evidence has since been reported by several studies of children at high risk of dyslexia before formal schooling begins (Byrne, Fielding-Barnsley, Ashley, & Larsen, 1997;Gallagher, Frith, & Snowling, 2000;Locke et al, 1997;Lyytinen, Poikkeus, Laakso, Eklund, & Lyytinen, 2001;Lefly & Pennington 1996). However, it needs to be borne in mind that this method of recruitment is very different from that used in more conventional group studies that have selected children who fulfill ÔdiscrepancyÕ and other more conventional criteria for reading disability.…”
Section: Dyslexia Across the Life-spanmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Gallagher et al (2000) found (consistent with Scarborough) that preschool syntactic ability was a significant predictor of reading at 8 years. Lyytinen, Poikkeus, Laakso, Eklund, and Lyytinen (2001) reported that maximum sentence length at age two and object naming and inflectional morphology skills at 3;6 years were less well developed in a group of at-risk children compared to normal controls.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%