2015
DOI: 10.1111/flan.12163
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Language Deficits in Poor L2 Comprehenders: The Simple View

Abstract: The simple view of reading (SVR) model proposes that reading comprehension is the product of word decoding and language comprehension, and that both components make independent contributions to reading skill (Gough & Tunmer, 1986). The model posits that there are good readers and three types of poor readers—dyslexic, hyperlexic, and garden variety—who exhibit different profiles of strengths and/or deficits in word decoding and language comprehension. In this study, 165 first‐ and second‐year high school studen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

5
18
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

2
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
(70 reference statements)
5
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Likewise, by the end of third‐year Spanish, U.S. students’ Spanish reading comprehension ( SS = 53.5, < first percentile) and listening comprehension ( SS = 45.3, < first percentile) skills were three to four standard deviations below their Spanish word decoding skills ( SS = 105.9, 65th percentile) when compared to those of fifth‐grade native Spanish speakers. These findings mirror those of previous studies with a different group of monolingual English high school students completing first‐ and second‐year Spanish courses in which the students displayed significantly stronger Spanish word decoding than Spanish reading comprehension (Sparks, ; Sparks & Patton, ). In L1 reading research, readers with this profile are rare and have been called hyperlexic because their very weak reading and language comprehension are unexpected given their strong word decoding skills (Gough & Tunmer, ; Hoover & Gough, ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Likewise, by the end of third‐year Spanish, U.S. students’ Spanish reading comprehension ( SS = 53.5, < first percentile) and listening comprehension ( SS = 45.3, < first percentile) skills were three to four standard deviations below their Spanish word decoding skills ( SS = 105.9, 65th percentile) when compared to those of fifth‐grade native Spanish speakers. These findings mirror those of previous studies with a different group of monolingual English high school students completing first‐ and second‐year Spanish courses in which the students displayed significantly stronger Spanish word decoding than Spanish reading comprehension (Sparks, ; Sparks & Patton, ). In L1 reading research, readers with this profile are rare and have been called hyperlexic because their very weak reading and language comprehension are unexpected given their strong word decoding skills (Gough & Tunmer, ; Hoover & Gough, ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…In L1 reading research, readers with this profile are rare and have been called hyperlexic because their very weak reading and language comprehension are unexpected given their strong word decoding skills (Gough & Tunmer, ; Hoover & Gough, ). In the Sparks () study as well as the current study, the large majority of monolingual English students displayed a hyperlexic profile. In another study, Sparks and Luebbers () found that almost all monolingual U.S. high school students enrolled in FL courses could have been labeled with a reading “disability” when compared to native Spanish speaker norms because their Spanish word decoding skills were so much stronger than their Spanish reading comprehension skills—i.e., their Spanish word decoding skills ranged from two to four standard deviations higher than Spanish reading comprehension.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 54%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In these studies, U.S. high school students’ Spanish vocabulary and listening comprehension skills were found to be similar to the average native Spanish speaker at a 2½‐ to 3‐year‐old level, even after 3 years of Spanish courses (Sparks & Luebbers, ; Sparks et al, b). Other studies have shown that despite IDs in L1 achievement, U.S. high school students learned to decode an alphabetic L2 (Spanish) well but still met criteria for a L2 reading “disability” because they exhibited extraordinarily poor reading comprehension and listening comprehension skills, largely because of extremely limited L2 vocabulary acquisition (Sparks, ; Sparks & Luebbers, ; Sparks et al, ). The constraint on L2 learning, in particular vocabulary learning, for U.S. students in high school is the social context in which the learning takes place—that is, most live in a home and a community where the target language is not spoken.…”
Section: Pedagogical Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%