2014
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01395
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?

Abstract: In three studies, we explored the retention and transfer of tool-making knowledge, learnt from an adult demonstration, to other temporal and task contexts. All studies used a variation of a task in which children had to make a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tall transparent tube. Children who failed to innovate the hook tool independently saw a demonstration. In Study 1, we tested children aged 4–6 years (N = 53) who had seen the original demonstration 3 months earlier. Performance was excellent at the … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
24
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
2
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…So this partial effect of apparatus that do not share the same technical principles suggests that technical analysis is done in the here and now. This is consistent with the results of Beck et al (2014), who reported no transfer of knowledge for the Hook task with different means and materials. Another explanation could be that there is an effect of complexity between the different mechanical problems used, since performance in the Sloping task is clearly inferior to that obtained in the other apparatus, whatever the age group.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…So this partial effect of apparatus that do not share the same technical principles suggests that technical analysis is done in the here and now. This is consistent with the results of Beck et al (2014), who reported no transfer of knowledge for the Hook task with different means and materials. Another explanation could be that there is an effect of complexity between the different mechanical problems used, since performance in the Sloping task is clearly inferior to that obtained in the other apparatus, whatever the age group.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Recent studies have, however, reported a striking dissociation between these early skills and the subsequent emergence of tool-making abilities in children. Studies have shown on multiple occasions that making a functional tool independently (i.e., without any demonstration, called “tool innovation” below) is nearly impossible in healthy children before the age of 5 years (Beck et al, 2011, 2014; Cutting et al, 2011) irrespective of their socio-cultural environment (Nielsen et al, 2014). The task commonly employed consists in recovering a target that is lodged inside a transparent tube and that cannot be reached directly by hand (e.g., the Hook task, Unbending task).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The experimental procedure of the hook task included one warm‐up and three tool‐making phases. A similar procedure was used by Cutting et al () [also see Beck et al () for the wooden sticks or dowels]. In this study, the wooden sticks were preferred as they could motivate dyadic interaction.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…In the two studies reported here, we aim to reveal social, representational, and creative factors underlying preschoolers’ tool‐making ability. We used Beck et al’s (, ) hook task, with wooden sticks. Individual children and/or dyads were given the opportunity to solve the task in one of three experimental phases.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%