2018
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12709
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Is there a direct relation between the development of vocabulary and grammar?

Abstract: Previous studies of individual differences have revealed strong correlations between children's vocabulary and grammatical abilities, and these data have been used to support theoretical accounts positing direct developmental relations between these two areas of language. However, between-person differences do not necessarily reflect intra-individual dynamics. Thus, in the present study, we analysed longitudinal data from three annual assessments of vocabulary and grammar in 217 children (M = 4 years and 3 mon… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
21
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
(141 reference statements)
5
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This finding is consistent with other recent reports that have documented links between early implicit eye gaze language assessments, such as Head-turn Preference Procedure (Newman, Ratner, Jusczyk, Jusczyk & Dow, 2006), Looking While Listening (e.g., Fernald & Marchman, 2012), and IPL (e.g., Naigles et al , 2011), and later language. Our findings are also an important extension of numerous findings showing links between early and later standardized tests (most recently Brinchmann, Braeken & Lyster, 2019), now with a psycholinguistic task – categorical induction – as the outcome. While standardized tests have been designed to categorize individuals on macro level skills (e.g., IQ, language) they are not finely honed on particular skills.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 82%
“…This finding is consistent with other recent reports that have documented links between early implicit eye gaze language assessments, such as Head-turn Preference Procedure (Newman, Ratner, Jusczyk, Jusczyk & Dow, 2006), Looking While Listening (e.g., Fernald & Marchman, 2012), and IPL (e.g., Naigles et al , 2011), and later language. Our findings are also an important extension of numerous findings showing links between early and later standardized tests (most recently Brinchmann, Braeken & Lyster, 2019), now with a psycholinguistic task – categorical induction – as the outcome. While standardized tests have been designed to categorize individuals on macro level skills (e.g., IQ, language) they are not finely honed on particular skills.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 82%
“…One potentially important factor that we did not consider in the present study is the home literacy environment and the extent to which reading and writing in the HL and/or the SL correlates with SES and in turn affects children’s language skills. Brinchmann, Braeken, and Lyster (2019), for instance, argue that the home literacy environment accounts for individual differences in vocabulary and grammar in preschool monolingual children. A hypothesis for future research is whether (and to what extent) the amount and diversity of literacy activities explain the relationship between SES and language proficiency in bilingual children.…”
Section: Summary and Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One potentially important factor that we did not consider in the present study is the home literacy environment and the extent to which reading and writing in the home language and/or the school language correlates with SES and in turn affects children's language skills. Brinchmann, Braeken, and Lyster (2019) Future research will be needed to determine if this is the case in older age groups as well.…”
Section: Language Dominance Thresholdsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The sum of this experience with language (their "input") is the basis for their lexical, grammatical, and sociolinguistic development. Much developmental language research focuses on the value of child-directed speech (CDS) in particular as a tailored source of linguistic input that can boost lexical and syntactic development (Bates & Goodman, 1997;Brinchmann, Braeken & Lyster, 2019;Frank, Braginsky, Yurovsky & Marchman, in press;Hart & Risley, 1995;Hoff, 2003;Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea & Hedges, 2010;Lieven, Pine & Baldwin, 1997;Marchman, Martínez-Sussmann & Dale, 2004;Snow, 1977;Weisleder & Fernald, 2013). However, we have also known for decades that children's language environmentse.g., who is around and talking about what to whomvary dramatically within and across families, and that children in some communities hear very little directed talk without any apparent delays in their linguistic development (e.g., Brown, 2011;Brown & Gaskins, 2014;de León, 2011;Gaskins, 2006;Ochs, 1988;Ochs & Schieffelin, 1984;Rogoff, Paradise, Arauz, Correa-Chávez & Angelillo, 2003;Schieffelin, 1990).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%