2010
DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2010.488074
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Is reasoning from counterfactual antecedents evidence for counterfactual reasoning?

Abstract: In most developmental studies the only error children could make on counterfactual tasks was to answer with the current state of affairs. It was concluded that children who did not show this error are able to reason counterfactually. However, children might have avoided this error by using basic conditional reasoning (Rafetseder, Cristi-Vargas, & Perner, 2010). Basic conditional reasoning takes an antecedent, which like in counterfactual reasoning can be counter to fact, and combines it with a conditional (or … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
20
0
3

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
(35 reference statements)
2
20
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…These studies primarily required children to reason about counterfactual conditionals from counterfactual antecedents, without having to incorporate other aspects of reality into their counterfactual representation (Rafetseder & Perner, ) and without having to consider multiple possibilities simultaneously (Beck & Riggs, ). Studies that have required children to consider multiple possibilities (e.g., reality and a counterfactual possibility) have found that children do not succeed until age 5 or 6 (Beck & Guthrie, ; Beck et al., ; Perner et al., ; Rafetseder & Perner, ). Those that have required children to exercise the nearest possible world constraint have found that children do not pass until as late as adolescence (Rafetseder et al., , ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These studies primarily required children to reason about counterfactual conditionals from counterfactual antecedents, without having to incorporate other aspects of reality into their counterfactual representation (Rafetseder & Perner, ) and without having to consider multiple possibilities simultaneously (Beck & Riggs, ). Studies that have required children to consider multiple possibilities (e.g., reality and a counterfactual possibility) have found that children do not succeed until age 5 or 6 (Beck & Guthrie, ; Beck et al., ; Perner et al., ; Rafetseder & Perner, ). Those that have required children to exercise the nearest possible world constraint have found that children do not pass until as late as adolescence (Rafetseder et al., , ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is also worth mentioning that one study by Rafetseder and Perner () found that children were able to engage in counterfactual reasoning by the age of 6. They gave children change‐of‐location counterfactual scenarios, in which a character was in a typical or atypical location when he was called to another location.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although the gain of inhibitory control might serve as an explanation for why children stop committing the reality bias on false belief and counterfactual questions (see also Rafetseder & Perner, 2010), it hardly explains why even older children give wrong answers on our critical conditions. The interference of reality with what needs to be counterfactually assumed is the same among all conditions; the girl comes instead of the boy, and so the candy would be somewhere else than where it really is.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In their study, children received antecedent and consequent counterfactual tasks. In the consequent version, they were asked counterfactual questions such as “If there had not been a fire, where would Peter be?” These are tasks that even very young children can solve correctly (Riggs et al, 1998), most likely because they are using simpler reasoning strategies (Rafetseder & Perner, 2010). In the antecedent tasks, children were asked to imagine that they had made the floor dirty.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is not to say, however, that mental object substitution implies that toddlers can construct and compare multiple scenarios as typically required for effective episodic foresight . Only from around ages three to four do children show signs of counterfactual thinking, allowing them to contrast an event that did happen with a similar event that did not happen, and some data point to an even later time of emergence …”
Section: The Components Of Mental Scenario Buildingmentioning
confidence: 99%