2014
DOI: 10.1086/677562
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Is It Still Working? Task Difficulty Promotes a Rapid Wear-Off Bias in Judgments of Pharmacological Products

Abstract: JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, lay theories can lead to warranted or unwarranted inferences about a medication's efficacy (Ilyuk, Block, & Faro, 2014; Ilyuk, Irmak, et al, 2014). These inferences, however, may lead to suboptimal decisions.…”
Section: External Cues and Consumer Medication Choicementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, lay theories can lead to warranted or unwarranted inferences about a medication's efficacy (Ilyuk, Block, & Faro, 2014; Ilyuk, Irmak, et al, 2014). These inferences, however, may lead to suboptimal decisions.…”
Section: External Cues and Consumer Medication Choicementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, and more importantly, we show the downstream consequences of this association on consumers' perception of products' lasting benefits. Although this dimension of product performance is highly predictive of purchases (Faro, 2010; Ilyuk, Block, & Faro, 2014), it has received relatively little research attention. Moreover, marketing messages often contain round numbers, but research on numerical cognition has focused primarily on documenting positive effects of precise numbers, including increases in perceived credibility (Schindler & Yalch, 2006), accuracy (Zhang & Schwarz, 2012), and competence (Xie & Kronrod, 2012).…”
Section: Theoretical Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This reasoning is in line with the limited yet growing body of research showing that efficacy perceptions and experiences are indeed highly malleable and often affected by product-, marketer-, and consumer-related factors that have little to do a product's actual efficacy. For example, prior research has shown that such factors may include price (Samper and Schwartz 2013), the product's origin (Wang et al 2010), the manufacturer's profitability information (Posavac et al 2010), the presence of negative product attributes (e.g., side effects; Kramer et al 2012), and consumers' concurrent activities (Ilyuk et al 2014a). Importantly, these factors have been shown to affect not only inferences about product efficacy, but also actual product experiences (e.g., increased mental acuity) in a placebo-like manner.…”
Section: Theoretical Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus in addition to adjusting their use of a target brand at their discretion or ceasing consumption of the product in general, consumers may also switch to competing brands. Third, consumers' inferences about-and actual experiences of-product efficacy are highly malleable and often biased by factors that have little to do with a product's actual efficacy (Chae, Li, and Zhu 2013;Faro 2010;Ilyuk, Block, and Faro 2014a;Kramer et al 2012;Posavac et al 2010;Samper and Schwartz 2013;Wang, Keh, and Bolton 2010;Wright et al 2013; for a review, see Ilyuk et al 2014b). As such, exploring the effects of marketing actions on consumers' efficacy perceptions is crucial.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%