2012
DOI: 10.1007/s11251-012-9224-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Investigating the mechanisms of learning from a constrained preparation for future learning activity

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
13
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
2
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, the observed effect does not seem to reflect differences in students’ confidence in their task‐specific skills, as we found no differences between students’ confidence in their experimental findings. Instead, this effect might be similar to what Siler, Klahr, and Price () found when they engaged students in CVS tasks prior to CVS training in order to prepare them for future learning. Students might develop some understanding of the materials and the nature of the task during training even when they do not receive feedback or guidance.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…Moreover, the observed effect does not seem to reflect differences in students’ confidence in their task‐specific skills, as we found no differences between students’ confidence in their experimental findings. Instead, this effect might be similar to what Siler, Klahr, and Price () found when they engaged students in CVS tasks prior to CVS training in order to prepare them for future learning. Students might develop some understanding of the materials and the nature of the task during training even when they do not receive feedback or guidance.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…A similar outcome can be expected when considering the continued-influence effect (Johnson & Seifert, 1994): Learners tend to stick to their own suboptimal solution instead of adopting the directly instructed canonical one. Similarly, Siler, Klahr, and Price (2013) found that students who applied the suboptimal engineering approach instead of the canonical science approach to designing experiments did not benefit from the preparation phase. However, research on productive failure (e.g., Kapur, 2010Kapur, , 2012Kapur & Bielaczyc, 2012;Loibl & Rummel, 2014a) shows that initial problem-solving activities can be effective even though invented solutions to problems are often suboptimal or even false (see Schmidt et al, 1989 for similar findings).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other studies like this have shown that children presented with type A instruction remembered and used what they learned about CVS in substantially different contexts (i.e., they transferred their CVS knowledge) (25), and they retained it for several months, and even several years, after their instruction (26)(27)(28). Moreover, the explicit steps involved in our type A instruction have been transformed into a computer tutor that is as effective as a human tutor for CVS (29).…”
Section: Operational Definitions and Shifting Terminology-a Cautionarmentioning
confidence: 68%