2014
DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.2787284
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intuitive and Reflective Responses in Philosophy

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3
1
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 289 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This pre-registered study was based on a prior study (Appendix; Byrd, 2014) that was motivated in large part by hypotheses generated from the above-mentioned evidence (1-3 below) as well as potentially competing hypotheses posed by anonymous reviewers and fellow philosophers (4-5).…”
Section: The Current Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This pre-registered study was based on a prior study (Appendix; Byrd, 2014) that was motivated in large part by hypotheses generated from the above-mentioned evidence (1-3 below) as well as potentially competing hypotheses posed by anonymous reviewers and fellow philosophers (4-5).…”
Section: The Current Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…So far only a relatively small study of philosophers and their reflection test performance has been published (Yaden & Anderson, 2021), but this and prior work on the issue used less than optimal reflection tests (Byrd, 2014;Appendix). So there is still a need for a larger conceptual replication and extension of existing findings involving both laypeople and philosophers using both original and state-of-the-art reflection tests as well as measures of both intuitions about particular thought experiments and acceptance of more general philosophical views.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Across several studies, Schwitzgebel and Rust (2010) found that ethicists were no more likely to exhibit various moral behaviors than their colleagues who are professors in other areas of philosophy. Some studies show that the number of philosophy courses one has taken is predictive of better performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT; Byrd, 2014;Frederick, 2005;Livengood et al, 2010), which measures one's tendency to engage in more careful analytic, as opposed to intuitive, modes of thinking. These findings focus on professional philosophers and help to characterize this population, but they still do not address the psychological factors that might impact their views.…”
Section: Experimental Philosophymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Notably, much of this work shows how intuitions are often sensitive to factors that are not normative (i.e. truth-tracking) and hence are best described as cognitive biases (Andow 2015;Byrd 2014;Livengood et al 2010;Pinillos et al 2011;Schwitzgebel and Cushman 2015). This work also indicates that philosophers tend to more carefully 'check' their intuitions with deliberate, reflective thought than do non-philosophersa finding which is hardly surprising given the goals of philosophical education, but nonetheless significant and gratifying to see empirically supported (e.g., Andow 2015;Byrd 2014;Livengood et al 2010).…”
Section: X-phi Reconsideredmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…truth-tracking) and hence are best described as cognitive biases (Andow 2015;Byrd 2014;Livengood et al 2010;Pinillos et al 2011;Schwitzgebel and Cushman 2015). This work also indicates that philosophers tend to more carefully 'check' their intuitions with deliberate, reflective thought than do non-philosophersa finding which is hardly surprising given the goals of philosophical education, but nonetheless significant and gratifying to see empirically supported (e.g., Andow 2015;Byrd 2014;Livengood et al 2010). Some influential work in X-phi has also shed new light on the psychology of social and ethical reasoning, in particular judgements of intentionality and responsibility (Knobe 2003;Shaun and Knobe 2007).…”
Section: X-phi Reconsideredmentioning
confidence: 99%