1964
DOI: 10.1177/001316446402400407
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intraindividual and Interindividual Positional Preference Response Styles in Ability Tests

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

1970
1970
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“… 11. Other position effects in test taking appeared in the literature in earlier decades. Some authors report a preference for early responses (Clark, 1956; Gustav, 1963), some for later responses (Marcus, 1963), some for neither (Hopkins & Hopkins, 1964), and some for both (Atwell & Wells, 1937; McNamara & Weitzman, 1945). Generally speaking, “The few studies on positional bias … are inconclusive because of methodological and conceptual problems” (Fagley, 1987, p. 95). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“… 11. Other position effects in test taking appeared in the literature in earlier decades. Some authors report a preference for early responses (Clark, 1956; Gustav, 1963), some for later responses (Marcus, 1963), some for neither (Hopkins & Hopkins, 1964), and some for both (Atwell & Wells, 1937; McNamara & Weitzman, 1945). Generally speaking, “The few studies on positional bias … are inconclusive because of methodological and conceptual problems” (Fagley, 1987, p. 95). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However when subjects have been confronted with examination tasks of a meaningful nature the findings of various researchers have been in conflict. Gustav (1963) and McNamara and Weitzman (1945) found significant positional response tendencies; contrary evidence was revealed in studies by Marcus (1963) and Hopkins and Hopkins (1964). This research was an attempt at clarifying these contradictory results.…”
mentioning
confidence: 59%
“…The few studies on positional response bias in multiplechoice achievement tests and classroom tests are inconclusive because of methodological and conceptual problems. The methodological problems included failure to randomize the position of keyed responses (e.g., Clark, 1956;Gustav, 1963;Wevrick, 1962); inclusion of unequal numbers of a-, b-, c-, and d-keyed items {e.g., Hopkins & Hopkins, 1964); failure to consider the effects of visual cues provided by answer sheets (e.g., Cronbach, 1950;Marcus, 1963); and violation of statistical assumptions (e.g., Marcus;McNamara & Weitzman, 1945;Rapaport & Berg, 1955).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%