Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
1994
DOI: 10.1163/156853994x00145
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intra-Group Vocal Behaviour in Wild Golden Lion Tamarins, Leontopithecus Rosalia: Honest Communication of Individual Activity

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1995
1995
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Despite the aforementioned differences, the overall form of food transfer behavior was similar in wild and reintroduced animals: immatures solicit with conspicuous signals and behavior and receive most transfers, and transfers of naturally occurring foods were biased toward require prey and some fruits. Adults occasionally offered food with a food-transfer call, which seems to be limited to this context [see also Boinski et al, 1994], and/or by sitting with an outstretched forearm [Brown & Mack, 1978;Hoage, 1982]. The form of the call is not stereotyped nor was its occurrence as common as previously suggested for captive tamarins [Brown & Mack, 1978;Hoage, 1982;Rapaport, 1997].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Despite the aforementioned differences, the overall form of food transfer behavior was similar in wild and reintroduced animals: immatures solicit with conspicuous signals and behavior and receive most transfers, and transfers of naturally occurring foods were biased toward require prey and some fruits. Adults occasionally offered food with a food-transfer call, which seems to be limited to this context [see also Boinski et al, 1994], and/or by sitting with an outstretched forearm [Brown & Mack, 1978;Hoage, 1982]. The form of the call is not stereotyped nor was its occurrence as common as previously suggested for captive tamarins [Brown & Mack, 1978;Hoage, 1982;Rapaport, 1997].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…Two observers recorded vocalizations and behavioral context [Boinski et al, 1994]. "Observer 1" recorded all vocalizations of the focal animal on one channel of a Marantz PMD 430 cassette recorder while narrating into a lapel microphone connected to a second channel the time of day, the behavior of the focal animal, its location in reference to the group, and the general activity of the group (e.g., foraging, traveling, resting, etc.)…”
Section: Design and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Relationships between vocal signalling and social cohesion, and between certain vocal signals and specific social interactions, have been documented in detail for many non-human primate species with complex social groups (e.g. chacma baboons, Papio hamadryas ursinus [25,26]; white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus [27]; golden lion tamarins, Leontopithecus rosalia [28]; (reviews in [17,29]).…”
Section: Social Complexity As a Driver Of Vocal Complexitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many primate species rely on vocal communication to mediate social interactions (Epple, 1968; Seyfarth et al, 1980; Boinski, 1993; Boinski et al, 1994; Clark and Wrangham, 1994; Vitale et al, 2003; Arnold and Zuberbühler, 2006), and the complexity of their call repertoires appears to be a function of adaptive pressure (Stephan and Zuberbühler, 2008). In spite of the importance of primate vocal communication, our understanding of its neural underpinnings is still quite limited.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%