2016
DOI: 10.1080/09500693.2016.1183265
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intersection of argumentation and the use of multiple representations in the context of socioscientific issues

Abstract: Using multiple representations and argumentation are two fundamental processes in science. With the advancements of information communication technologies, these two processes are blended more so than ever before. However, little is known about how these two processes interact with each other in student learning. Hence, we conducted a design-based study in order to distill the relationship between these two processes. Specifically, we designed a learning unit on nuclear energy and implemented it with a group o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
17
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 102 publications
(141 reference statements)
1
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Under this category, the two most prevalent examples of claims were whether a nuclear power plant should be built in the participants' region and whether specific types of genetically modified organisms should be produced or allowed. In studies that had been assigned an adoption code, learners did not tend to argue whether a specific design should be adopted—for example, by arguing that a nuclear power plant with boiling water reactors versus pressurized heavy water reactors should be built—but rather they made claims based on the ramifications of a generalized technology as a whole—for example, by arguing that a generic nuclear power plant with non‐specified components should or should not be built in a particular place (e.g., Namdar & Shen, ; Ozturk & Yilmaz‐Tuzun, ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Under this category, the two most prevalent examples of claims were whether a nuclear power plant should be built in the participants' region and whether specific types of genetically modified organisms should be produced or allowed. In studies that had been assigned an adoption code, learners did not tend to argue whether a specific design should be adopted—for example, by arguing that a nuclear power plant with boiling water reactors versus pressurized heavy water reactors should be built—but rather they made claims based on the ramifications of a generalized technology as a whole—for example, by arguing that a generic nuclear power plant with non‐specified components should or should not be built in a particular place (e.g., Namdar & Shen, ; Ozturk & Yilmaz‐Tuzun, ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To date, research on argumentation has emphasized how argument structure is practiced through writing (McNeill, Lizotte, Krajcik, & Marx, ), reading (von der Mühlen, Richter, Schmid, & Berthold, ), or oral discourse (Christodoulou & Osborne, ); how the practices of argument construction, critique, justification, and evaluation are performed (Berland & Reiser, ; Chen, Park, & Hand, 2016; Ford, ); how informal reasoning on socio‐scientific issues is progressed (Sadler, ; Venville & Dawson, ; Zangori, Peel, Kinslow, Friedrichsen, & Sadler, ; Zeidler, Herman, Ruzek, Linder, & Lin, ); how multiple models of representation are adapted to articulate and debate arguments for knowledge development (Namdar & Shen, ; Waldrip & Prain, ); how teachers’ conceptions (Sampson & Blanchard, ) and knowledge (McNeill & Knight, ; Suh & Park, ) for using argumentation relate to their pedagogical decisions; and how teachers orchestrate diverse ideas, alternative concepts, and counterarguments to stimulate productive thinking (Chen, Hand, & Norton‐Meier, 2017; Chin & Osborne, ). Relatively little research has investigated argumentation as an enterprise of managing uncertainty, the role of uncertainty in argumentation, or how teachers manage uncertainty in ways that contribute to students’ conceptual development.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the learning process, constructing the infographics helps students to develop non-linear ways of thinking, reasoning and representing (Wu & Puntambekar, 2012). Researchers also determined that using multiple representations, such as infographics and modelling, helps learners to develop scientific argumentation and writing skills (Demirbag & Gunel, 2014;Namdar & Shen, 2016), establish relationship between concepts (Jonassen & Carr, 2000), determine representational adequacy and enhance their representational competence (Gebre & Polman, 2016;diSessa, 2004).…”
Section: Infographics As Cognitive Toolsmentioning
confidence: 99%