2000
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1188
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interresponse times in serial recall: Effects of intraserial repetition.

Abstract: The authors examined the effects of intraserial repetition on multitrial serial learning of random consonant lists, analyzing both learning rates and perfect trial inten-esponse times (IRTs). Lists varied along 3 dimensions: list length, presence or absence of a repeated element, and lag between repeated elements. After achieving a forward-recall criterion on a given list, participants (N = 20) attempted backward recall. At small lags, IRTs between the repeated elements were very short (compared with IRTs from… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
46
0
2

Year Published

2002
2002
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
(104 reference statements)
3
46
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The authors observed a significant reduction of memory span under the combined-tasks condition (from 4.75 to 3, approximately), but 2 It could be argued that presenting the memory items for 1,500 ms or the stimuli to be processed for 1 s or more, as we did here, does not allow one to consider that time was controlled. It is true that items to be recalled are usually presented for 1,000 ms (Conlin & Gathercole, 2006;Kahana & Jacobs, 2000;Oberauer et al, 2004;Rosen & Engle, 1997;Shah & Miyake, 1996), but it should be remembered that this duration is generally used for convenience and out of habit rather than being theoretically grounded. Longer presentations are not rare (1,250 ms in Hale, Myerson, Rhee, Weiss, & Abrams, 1996; 1,500 ms in Miyake, Friedman, Rettinger, Shah, & Hegarty, 2001; 2,000 ms in Duff & Logie, 2001).…”
Section: The Relationships Between Processing and Storagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The authors observed a significant reduction of memory span under the combined-tasks condition (from 4.75 to 3, approximately), but 2 It could be argued that presenting the memory items for 1,500 ms or the stimuli to be processed for 1 s or more, as we did here, does not allow one to consider that time was controlled. It is true that items to be recalled are usually presented for 1,000 ms (Conlin & Gathercole, 2006;Kahana & Jacobs, 2000;Oberauer et al, 2004;Rosen & Engle, 1997;Shah & Miyake, 1996), but it should be remembered that this duration is generally used for convenience and out of habit rather than being theoretically grounded. Longer presentations are not rare (1,250 ms in Hale, Myerson, Rhee, Weiss, & Abrams, 1996; 1,500 ms in Miyake, Friedman, Rettinger, Shah, & Hegarty, 2001; 2,000 ms in Duff & Logie, 2001).…”
Section: The Relationships Between Processing and Storagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, to facilitate comparison of these two studies, we included trials only through the first perfect recall, rather than through all three consecutive perfect recalls, in our analyses of Kahana and Jacobs (2000). For Kahana and Jacobs's study (recall of consonants), the mean number of trials to learn the list was three, whereas the mean number for Kahana and Caplan's (2002) study (recall of words) was five.…”
Section: The Learning Curvementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In plotting the learning curves for recall of consonants (Kahana & Jacobs, 2000) and recall of words (Kahana & Caplan, 2002), we compared three scoring methods: strict positional scoring, relative order scoring, and lenient (item) scoring (Drewnowski & Murdock, 1980). Strict positional scoring counts a recalled list item as correct only if it is in the correct serial position.…”
Section: The Learning Curvementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This class of models, of which the LIST PARSE model is an example, has the advantages of being able to naturally account for certain data sets (e.g., response latency data; for a review: Farrell & Lewandowsky, 2004) that other models are incapable of accommodating, being conceptually simple, capable of self-organizing in a neural network context, and enjoying straight-forward biological interpretations. However, these models tend to (a) lack full treatments of item repetition phenomena (e.g., the Ranschburg effect, in which repeated items separated by more than one item are less likely to be recalled than distinct items, whereas less separation increases probability of recall, reviewed in Jacobs, 2000, andHenson, 1998a;however, see Bradski et al, 1994 for a partial treatment of repetition), and (b) fail to handle some of the positional error data for which other (positional) models have more ready explanations (e.g., the ability of Burgess & Hitch (1999) and Henson (1998b) to treat positional item interchanges across trials or temporal groupings). The limitations in (b) suggest that direct positional information of some sort may indeed be used in the coding of temporal order in the brain.…”
Section: Serial Recall Modelingmentioning
confidence: 99%