1996
DOI: 10.3758/bf03207549
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Internal structure of voicing categories in early infancy

Abstract: It is well established that young infants process speech in terms of perceptual categories that closely correspond to the phonetic categories of adult language users. Recently, Kuhl (1991) has provided evidence that this correspondence is not limited to the region of category boundaries: At least by 6-7 months of age, vowel categories of infants, like those of adults, have an internal perceptual structure. In the current experiments, which focused on a consonantal contrast, we found evidence of internally stru… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

2
46
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
2
46
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For a discussion of the ontogeny of internal category structure from various perspectives, see Kluender (1994), Kuhl (2000), and Miller and Eimas (1996).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For a discussion of the ontogeny of internal category structure from various perspectives, see Kluender (1994), Kuhl (2000), and Miller and Eimas (1996).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A subsequent study by Polka and Bohn (1996) provided a direct test of this hypothesis that perceptual asymmetries in infant vowel perception are due to the relatively more familiar vowels acting as 1 It should be noted that perceptual asymmetries have also been discussed by Miller and Eimas (1996) with respect to the perception of voicing categories. perceptual magnets.…”
Section: Asymmetries In Infant Vowel Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An anchor effect is present when there is increased discriminability and presumed expansion of the psychological space around the reference point, whereas a magnet effect is present when there is decreased discriminability and presumed shrinkage of psychological space around the reference point. Fourth, and consistent with a magnet effect, reference points may display asymmetries in similarity judgment and discrimination tasks (Krumhansl, 1978(Krumhansl, , 1979Miller & Eimas, 1996;Rosch, 1975;Tversky, 1977). Specifically, higher similarity (and lower discriminability) are in evidence when the reference point stimulus is the standard and the nonreference point stimulus is the comparison than when the converse is true.'…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The present report is concerned with the ontogeny of reference points-in particular, with whether reference points arise because of inherent biases in our perceptual and memory systems that may, in turn, allow for rapid learning of certain classes of information (see Walton & Bower, 1993). Evidence from phonetic categories indicates an early (i.e., prelinguistic) functional onset for reference points in the domain of speech (Kuhl, 1991;Miller & Eimas, 1996), but whether this is the case for categories in the domain of vision remains to be determined. The dimensions of form and orientation were chosen for investigation because ofevidence that ( I) reference points function in these domains for adults (Pomerantz, 1977;Rosch, 1975;Sebrechts & Garner, 1981) and (2) young infants develop category representations for simple geometric form classes and vertical and oblique orientations (reviewed in Quinn & Eimas, 1986), thereby allowing one to establish whether reference points serve as magnets or anchors within their respective classes.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%