2017
DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01072
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intermodal Attention Shifts in Multimodal Working Memory

Abstract: Additional information:Use policyThe full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that:• a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in DRO • the full-text is not changed in any way The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders.Pl… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
(57 reference statements)
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Third, the reliability of the cues could vary from 50% to 100%, while reliability increased the retro-cue effects ( Shimi et al, 2013 ; Gunseli et al, 2015 ). Fourth, there were cues in different sensory domain, including visual ( Landman, Spekreijse & Lamme, 2003 ; Griffin & Nobre, 2003 ), auditory ( Backer & Alain, 2012 ) and tactile ( Katus & Eimer, 2015 ), even crossmodal cues ( Katus, Grubert & Eimer, 2016 ). Fifth, the retro-cue could be presented either centrally or peripherally, when the effects were comparable ( Matsukura et al, 2014 ).…”
Section: Survey Methodologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Third, the reliability of the cues could vary from 50% to 100%, while reliability increased the retro-cue effects ( Shimi et al, 2013 ; Gunseli et al, 2015 ). Fourth, there were cues in different sensory domain, including visual ( Landman, Spekreijse & Lamme, 2003 ; Griffin & Nobre, 2003 ), auditory ( Backer & Alain, 2012 ) and tactile ( Katus & Eimer, 2015 ), even crossmodal cues ( Katus, Grubert & Eimer, 2016 ). Fifth, the retro-cue could be presented either centrally or peripherally, when the effects were comparable ( Matsukura et al, 2014 ).…”
Section: Survey Methodologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…CSDs were separately averaged across three adjacent electrodes contralateral and ipsilateral to the task-relevant side. Tactile contralateral delay activity (tCDA component) was measured at lateral central scalp regions (C3/4, FC3/4, CP3/4), and visual contralateral delay activity (CDA) was measured at lateral occipital regions (PO7/8, PO3/4, O1/2) (same electrodes as in prior work: Katus et al, 2017;Katus & Eimer, 2016;Katus & Eimer, 2018). Statistical tests were conducted on difference values of contra-minus ipsilateral ERPs, averaged between 300 and 1000 ms after sample onset (e.g, Vogel & Machizawa, 2004).…”
Section: Processing Of Eeg Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Tactile contralateral delay activity (tCDA component) was measured at lateral central scalp regions (C3/4, FC3/4, CP3/4), and visual contralateral delay activity (CDA) was measured at lateral occipital regions (PO7/8, PO3/4, O1/2). The electrode locations used to quantify tCDA and CDA components were identical to those employed in previous studies (Katus and Eimer 2016;Katus, Grubert, and Eimer 2017;Katus and Eimer 2018a;2018b). Separate CDA/tCDA components were computed for the delay periods following the two sample sets (S1 and S2), based on averaged CSDs obtained between 300 and 1000 ms following the onset of S1 or S2 (e.g., Katus Spline-interpolated topographical voltage maps were computed as follows:…”
Section: Artefact Rejection and Correctionmentioning
confidence: 99%