Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2019.104063
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interference patterns in subject-verb agreement and reflexives revisited: A large-sample study

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

9
139
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
2

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 106 publications
(164 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
9
139
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, there is no evidence that the intercept and slope adjustments are correlated: the estimated correlation is -0.14, with 95% credible interval [-0.64,0.44]. In a larger-sample replication attempt of this experiment [85], which had 181 participants instead of the original sample size of 40, the estimated correlation is -0.24, 95% credible interval [-0.71, 0.38]. Of course, this absence of a clear correlation does not imply there is none; it just means that we could not find support for the correlation explanation for the varying magnitudes of the facilitatory interference effect.…”
Section: Modeling Individual Differencesmentioning
confidence: 83%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, there is no evidence that the intercept and slope adjustments are correlated: the estimated correlation is -0.14, with 95% credible interval [-0.64,0.44]. In a larger-sample replication attempt of this experiment [85], which had 181 participants instead of the original sample size of 40, the estimated correlation is -0.24, 95% credible interval [-0.71, 0.38]. Of course, this absence of a clear correlation does not imply there is none; it just means that we could not find support for the correlation explanation for the varying magnitudes of the facilitatory interference effect.…”
Section: Modeling Individual Differencesmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…The participant-level estimates of the interference effect were computed from a hierarchical Bayesian model [86]. The average facilitatory interference effect is shown as a 95% credible interval (labeled Observed effect), and the range of predicted values from the activation model are also shown as a 95% credible interval (labeled Model prediction); the model predictions are computed using Approximate Bayesian Computation [85,87,88]. We see the predicted facilitatory interference effect even at the individual participant level, but different participants show varying magnitudes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Furthermore, although the N400 has been generally characterized as purely semantic, there is evidence that the mismatch in grammatical features (such as argument structure) can elicit an N400 effect (Chow et al, 2016); the N400 has also been found when syntactic reanalysis occurs (Bornkessel, McElree, Schlesewsky, & Friederici, 2004). A theoretical advantage of assuming that feature activation is not restricted to semantic features is that the extension of features allows for a single mechanism responsible for the access to memory for prediction generation and for the creation of dependencies with previous words in the sentence (as in Lewis & Vasishth, 2005;Engelmann, Jäger, & Vasishth, 2019;Jäger, Mertzen, Van Dyke, & Vasishth, 2019;Vasishth, Nicenboim, Engelmann, & Burchert, 2019, 11).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The interference effect considered here is a semantic one; future work will determine the extent to which this method can be applied to purely morphosyntactic interference effects, e.g., in subject-verb number agreement and reflexive binding (Dillon et al, 2013;Jäger et al, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%