1994
DOI: 10.1017/s030500090000934x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interactive communication between children: learning how to make language work in dialogue

Abstract: In this paper we investigate the development of interactive communication skills in 170 children aged seven to thirteen. Using a communication task that allows extended dialogues between pairs of young speakers, we are able to assess both the overall communicative success achieved by any pair; and from an analysis of the dialogues we can identify several interactive strategies that characterize older and more successful communicators. Successful communication involves the active involvement of both participant… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
29
1

Year Published

1998
1998
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
4
29
1
Order By: Relevance
“…taking the child's knowledge into account. For example, Anderson, Clark, and Mullin (1994) found that where speakers checked the addressee's understanding of new referents when introducing them, performance on the Map Task tended to be better. The second aim was to examine children's ability to maintain task focus.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…taking the child's knowledge into account. For example, Anderson, Clark, and Mullin (1994) found that where speakers checked the addressee's understanding of new referents when introducing them, performance on the Map Task tended to be better. The second aim was to examine children's ability to maintain task focus.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…And so on up the age range this flow is assumed to take place but only a longitudinal study could confirm such a hypothesis. However, results from a cross-sectional study by Anderson, Clark & Mullin (1994), using a more collaborative map task with pairs of same-aged children, indicate that there are as many as 25% of the oldest group of children (13 years) who are performing below the average level of the youngest children (7 years). This implies that there is a substantial minority of children who fail to improve their communicative skills with age.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…eye contact between speakers), and how these interacted with linguistic realisations (including speakers' use of contrastive stress). Subsequently, the Map Task has been widely used to support the study of spontaneous speech and communication by children (Anderson et al , 1992Doherty-Sneddon & Kent 1996) and adults Boyle et al 1994;Davies 1997;Kowtko 1997;Sotillo 1997). It has seen particular service in exploring the communication of atypical language users, including sleep-deprived soldiers (Bard et al 1996) and aphasic individuals (Anderson et al 1997;Beeke et al 1996;Merrison 2002).…”
Section: The Map Task: Data Generation and Epistemologymentioning
confidence: 99%